Advertisement

Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders

, Volume 43, Issue 6, pp 1423–1436 | Cite as

Linguistic Alignment in Adults with and Without Asperger’s Syndrome

  • Katie E. Slocombe
  • Ivan Alvarez
  • Holly P. Branigan
  • Tjeerd Jellema
  • Hollie G. Burnett
  • Anja Fischer
  • Yan Hei Li
  • Simon Garrod
  • Liat Levita
Original Paper

Abstract

Individuals with Asperger’s Syndrome (AS) often have difficulties with social interactions and conversations. We investigated if these difficulties could be attributable to a deficit in the ability to linguistically converge with an interlocutor, which is posited to be important for successful communication. To that end, participants completed two cooperative tasks with a confederate, which allowed us to measure linguistic alignment with the confederate in terms of lexical choice, syntactic structure and spatial frame of reference. There was no difference in the performance of individuals with AS and matched controls and both groups showed significant alignment with the confederate at all three levels. We conclude that linguistic alignment is intact in adults with AS engaged in structured, goal-directed social interactions.

Keywords

Linguistic alignment Asperger’s Syndrome Autism spectrum disorder Social communication Conversation Dialogue 

Notes

Acknowledgments

We are grateful to Kate Brook, Sara Shaikh, Francesca Woods, Steph Burchill and Fern Cosgrave for help with data collection and stimuli creation and to Paula Clarke for advice and training. This project was funded by the Department of Psychology, University of York Summer Bursary Scheme 2008 and 2009.

References

  1. Allen, M. L., Haywood, S., Rajendran, G., & Branigan, H. (2011). Evidence for syntactic alignment in children with autism. Developmental Science, 14(3), 540–548.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed., text revision). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Publishing.Google Scholar
  3. Ashwin, C., Baron-Cohen, S., Weelwright, S., O’Riordan, M., & Bullmore, E. T. (2007). Differential activation of the amygdala and the ‘social brain’ during face-processing in Asperger Syndrome. Neuropsychologia, 45(1), 2–14.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Attwood, T. (1997). Asperger syndrome: A guide for parents and professionals. London: Jessica Kingsley Publisher.Google Scholar
  5. Baron-Cohen, S. (1995). Mindblindness: An essay on autism and theory of mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  6. Begeer, S., Malle, B. F., Nieuwland, M. S., & Keysar, B. (2010). Using theory of mind to represent and take part in social interactions: Comparing individuals with high-functioning autism and typically developing controls. European Journal of Developmental Psychology, 7(1), 104–122.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Bortfeld, H., & Brennan, S. E. (1997). Use and acquisition of idiomatic expressions in referring by native and non-native speakers. Discourse Processes, 23, 119–147.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Branigan, H. P., Pickering, M. J., & Cleland, A. A. (2000). Syntactic coordination in dialogue. Cognition, 75, B13–B25.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Branigan, H. P., Pickering, M. J., Pearson, J., & McLean, J. F. (2010). Linguistic alignment between humans and computers. Journal of Pragmatics, 42, 2355–2368.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Branigan, H. P., Pickering, M. J., Pearson, J., McLean, J. F., & Brown, A. (2011). The role of beliefs in lexical alignment: Evidence from dialogs with humans and computers. Cognition, 121(1), 41–57.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Brennan, S. E., & Clark, H. H. (1996). Conceptual pacts and lexical choice in conversation. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 22(6), 1482–1493.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Chang, F., Dell, G. S., & Bock, K. (2006). Becoming syntactic. Psychological Review, 113(2), 234–272.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Chartrand, T. L., & Bargh, J. (1999). The chameleon effect: The perception-behavior link and social interaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76(6), 893–910.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Clark, H. H. (1996). Using language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Colle, L., Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., & van der Lely, H. (2008). Narrative discourse in adults with high-functioning autism or asperger syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 38(1), 28–40.Google Scholar
  16. Di Pellegrino, G., Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L., et al. (1992). Understanding motor events—A neurophysiological study. Experimental Brain Research, 91(1), 176–180.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Dunn, L., Dunn, L., Whetton, C., & Burley, J. (1997). British Picture Vocabulary Scale-II. Slough: NFER-Nelson.Google Scholar
  18. Ferreira, V. S., Bock, K., Wilson, M. P., & Cohen, N. J. (2008). Memory for syntax despite amnesia. Psychological Science, 19(9), 940–946.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Garrod, S., & Anderson, A. (1987). Saying what you mean in dialogue: a study in conceptual and semantic co-ordination. Cognition, 27(2), 181–218.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Garrod, S., & Clark, A. (1993). The development of dialog coordination skills in schoolchildren. Language and Cognitive Processes, 8(1), 101–126.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Garrod, S., & Pickering, M. J. (2004). Why is conversation so easy? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8(1), 8–11.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Ghaziuddin, M., & Gerstein, L. (1996). Pedantic speaking style differentiates Asperger syndrome from high-functioning autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 26(6), 585–595.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Giles, H., et al. (1992). Accommodation theory: communication, context and consequences. In H. Giles, et al. (Eds.), Contexts of accommodation (pp. 1–68). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  24. Harms, M. B., Martin, A., & Wallace, G. L. (2010). Facial emotion recognition in autism spectrum disorders: A review of behavioural and neuroimaging studies. Neuropsychology Review, 20, 290–322.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Hudson, M., Burnett, H. G., & Jellema, T. (2012). Anticipating action intentions in Autism Spectrum Disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. doi: 10.1007/s10803-011-1410-y).
  26. Jellema, T., Lorteije, J. A. M., van Rijn, S., van’t Wout, M., De Haan, E. H. F., Van Engeland, H., et al. (2009). Involuntary processing of social cues is compromised in Autism Spectrum Disorders. Autism Research, 2(4), 192–204.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Kamio, Y., Robins, D., Kelley, E., Swainson, B., & Fein, D. (2007). Atypical lexical/semantic processing in high-functioning autism spectrum disorders without early language delay. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37(6), 1116–1122.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Klin, A., Jones, W., Schultz, R., Volkmar, F., & Cohen, D. (2002). Defining and quantifying the social phenotype in autism. American Journal of Psychiatry, 159(6), 895–908.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Pardo, J. S. (2006). On phonetic convergence during conversational interaction. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 119, 2382.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Paul, R., Orlovski, S. M., Marcinko, H. C., & Volkmar, F. (2009). Conversational behaviors in youth with high-functioning ASD and Asperger Syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 39(1), 115–125.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Pickering, M. J., & Branigan, H. P. (1998). The representation of verbs: Evidence from syntactic priming in language production. Journal of Memory and Language, 39, 633–651.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Pickering, M. J., & Garrod, S. (2004). Towards a mechanistic psychology of dialogue. Behavioural and Brain Sciences, 27, 169–190.Google Scholar
  33. Rogers, S. J., Bennetto, L., McEvoy, R., & Pennington, B. F. (1996). Imitation and pantomime in high functioning adolescents with autism spectrum disorders. Child Development, 67, 2060–2073.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Rogers, S. J., Hepburn, S. L., Stackhouse, T., & Wehner, E. (2003). Imitation performance in toddlers with autism and those with other developmental disorders. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 44(5), 763–781.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Rutter, M. (1978). Diagnosis and definition of childhood Autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 8, 139–161.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Schober, M. F. (1993). Spatial perspective-taking in conversation. Cognition, 47, 1–24.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Senju, A., Southgate, V., White, S., & Frith, U. (2009). Mindblind eyes: An absence of spontaneous theory of mind in Asperger Syndrome. Science, 325(5942), 883–885.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Smith, I. M., & Bryson, S. E. (1994). Imitation and action in Autism—A critical review. Psychological Bulletin, 116(2), 259–273.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Snodgrass, J. G., & Vanderwart, M. (1980). Standardized set of 260 pictures—Norms for name agreement, image agreement, familiarity, and visual complexity. Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Learning and Memory, 6(2), 174–215.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Tager-Flusberg, H., Paul, R., & Lord, C. (2005). Language and communication in autism. In F. Volkmar, R. Paul, A. Klin, & D. Cohen (Eds.), Handbook of autism and pervasive developmental disorders (3rd ed., Vol. 1, pp. 335–364). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  41. Van Baaren, R. B., Holland, R. W., Kawakami, K., & van Knippenberg, A. (2004). Mimicry and prosocial behaviour. Psychological Science, 15(1), 71–74.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Van Baaren, R. B., Holland, R. W., Steenaert, B., & van Knippenberg, A. (2003). Mimicry for money: Behavioural consequences of imitation. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 39(4), 393–398.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Van Eylen, L., Boets, B., Steyaert, J., Evers, K., Wagemans, J., & Noens, I. (2011). Cognitive flexibility in autism spectrum disorder: Explaining the inconsistencies? Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 5(4), 1390–1401.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Watson, M. E., Pickering, M. J., & Branigan, H. P. (2004). Alignment of reference frames in dialogue. In Proceedings of the 26th annual conference of the cognitive science society. Chicago.Google Scholar
  45. Williams, J. H. G., Whiten, A., Suddendorf, T., & Perrett, D. I. (2001). Imitation, mirror neurons and autism. Neuroscience and Behavioural Reviews, 25, 287–295.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Wong, T. K. W., Fung, P. C. W., Chua, S. E., & McAlonan, G. M. (2008). Abnormal spatiotemporal processing of emotional facial expressions in childhood autism: dipole source analysis of event-related potentials. European Journal of Neuroscience, 28(2), 407–416.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media New York 2012

Authors and Affiliations

  • Katie E. Slocombe
    • 1
  • Ivan Alvarez
    • 1
  • Holly P. Branigan
    • 2
  • Tjeerd Jellema
    • 3
  • Hollie G. Burnett
    • 3
  • Anja Fischer
    • 1
    • 5
  • Yan Hei Li
    • 1
  • Simon Garrod
    • 4
  • Liat Levita
    • 1
    • 6
  1. 1.Department of PsychologyUniversity of YorkYorkUK
  2. 2.Department of PsychologyUniversity of EdinburghEdinburghUK
  3. 3.Department of PsychologyUniversity of HullHullUK
  4. 4.Department of PsychologyUniversity of GlasgowGlasgowUK
  5. 5.Institute for Neuroimmunology and Clinical MS ResearchCenter for Molecular NeurobiologyHamburgGermany
  6. 6.Department of PsychologyUniversity of SheffieldSheffieldUK

Personalised recommendations