Advertisement

Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders

, Volume 43, Issue 2, pp 432–444 | Cite as

The Effects of Autism and Alexithymia on Physiological and Verbal Responsiveness to Music

  • Rory Allen
  • Rob Davis
  • Elisabeth Hill
Original Paper

Abstract

It has been suggested that individuals with autism will be less responsive to the emotional content of music than typical individuals. With the aim of testing this hypothesis, a group of high-functioning adults on the autism spectrum was compared with a group of matched controls on two measures of emotional responsiveness to music, comprising physiological and verbal measures. Impairment in participants ability to verbalize their emotions (type-II alexithymia) was also assessed. The groups did not differ significantly on physiological responsiveness, but the autism group was significantly lower on the verbal measure. However, inclusion of the alexithymia score as a mediator variable nullified this group difference, suggesting that the difference was due not to absence of underlying emotional responsiveness to music in autism, but to a reduced ability to articulate it.

Keywords

Autism Music Alexithymia Emotion 

Notes

Acknowledgments

The research reported in this paper was conducted by the first author as part of a program of PhD study, and is largely based on material contained in his PhD thesis. This program of study was self-funded by the first author. The third author was the first author’s PhD supervisor. The second author provided significant assistance with developing the equipment for carrying out the GSR experiments and the software for recording the results in a form available for subsequent analysis by the first author using standard statistical methods.

References

  1. Allen, R., & Heaton, P. (2010). Autism, music, and the therapeutic potential of music in alexithymia. Music Perception, 27(4), 251–261.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Allen, R., Hill, E., & Heaton, P. (2009). ‘Hath charms to soothe…’ An exploratory study of how high-functioning adults with ASD experience music. Autism, 13(1), 21–41.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th Ed., text Revision. Ed.). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.Google Scholar
  4. Bagby, R. M., Parker, J. D. A., & Taylor, G. J. (1994). The 20-item Toronto-Alexithymia-Scale.1. Item selection and cross-validation of the factor structure. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 38(1), 23–32.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator mediator variable distinction in social psychological-research—Conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51(6), 1173–1182.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Skinner, R., Martin, J., & Clubley, E. (2001). The Autism-spectrum quotient (AQ): Evidence from Asperger syndrome/high-functioning autism, males and females, scientists and mathematicians. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 31(1), 5–17.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Begeer, S., Koot, H. M., Rieffe, C., Terwogt, M. M., & Stegge, H. (2008). Emotional competence in children with autism: Diagnostic criteria and empirical evidence. Developmental Review, 28(3), 342–369.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Berthoz, S., & Hill, E. L. (2005). The validity of using self-reports to assess emotion regulation abilities in adults with autism spectrum disorder. European Psychiatry, 20(3), 291–298.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Bhatara, A. K., Quintin, E.-M., Heaton, P., Fombonne, E., & Levitin, D. J. (2009). The effect of music on social attribution in adolescents with autism spectrum disorders. Child Neuropsychology, 15(4), 375–396.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Bird, G., Silani, G., Brindley, R., White, S., Frith, U., & Singer, T. (2010). Empathic brain responses in insula are modulated by levels of alexithymia but not autism. Brain, 133, 1515–1525.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Blood, A. J., & Zatorre, R. J. (2001). Intensely pleasurable responses to music correlate with activity in brain regions implicated in reward and emotion. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 98(20), 11818–11823.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Carlson, S., Rama, P., Artchakov, D., & Linnankoski, I. (1997). Effects of music and white noise on working memory performance in monkeys. NeuroReport, 8(13), 2853–2856.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Dellacherie, D., Roy, M., Hugueville, L., Peretz, I., & Samson, S. (2010). The effect of musical experience on emotional self-reports and psychophysiological responses to dissonance. Psychophysiology, 48(3), 337–349.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Dunn, L., Dunn, L., Whetton, C., & Burley, J. (1997). British picture vocabulary scale. London: NFER-Nelson.Google Scholar
  15. Falkmer, M., Stuart, G. W., Danielsson, H., Bram, S., Lonebrink, M., & Falkmer, T. (2011). Visual acuity in adults with Asperger’s syndrome: No evidence for “Eagle-Eyed” vision. Biological Psychiatry, 70(9), 812–816.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Fritz, T., Jentschke, S., Gosselin, N., Sammler, D., Peretz, I., Turner, R., et al. (2009). Universal recognition of three basic emotions in music. Current Biology, 19(7), 573–576.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Hala, S., Pexman, P. M., & Glenwright, M. (2007). Priming the meaning of homographs in typically developing children and children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37(2), 329–340.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Harvey, D. I., Leybourne, S. J., & Taylor, A. M. R. (2009). Simple, robust, and powerful tests of the breaking trend hypothesis. Econometric Theory, 25(4), 995–1029.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Heaton, P. (2009). Speaking about music and the music of speech. Brain, 132(10), 2897–2899.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Heaton, P., Allen, R., Williams, K., Cummins, O., & Happe, F. (2008). Do social and cognitive deficits curtail musical understanding? Evidence from autism and Down syndrome. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 26, 171–182.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Heaton, P., Hermelin, B., & Pring, L. (1999). Can children with autistic spectrum disorders perceive affect in music? An experimental investigation. Psychological Medicine, 29(6), 1405–1410.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Heider, F., & Simmel, M. (1944). An experimental study of apparent behavior. The American Journal of Psychology, 57(2), 243–259.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Hill, E., Berthoz, S., & Frith, U. (2004). Brief report: Cognitive processing of own emotions in individuals with autistic spectrum disorder and in their relatives. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 34(2), 229–235.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Howell, D. (2012). Statistical methods for psychology (8th Ed.). Belmont, CA: Wadsworth.Google Scholar
  25. Huron, D. (2001). Is music an evolutionary adaptation? Biological Foundations of Music, 930, 43–61.Google Scholar
  26. iWorx. (2011). iWorx Psychophysiology Lab Manual. Retrieved 20 March 2012, from http://www.iworx.com/LabExercises/lockedexercises/LockedGSRANL.pdf.
  27. Juslin, P. N., & Sloboda, J. A. (2010). Handbook of music and emotion: Theory, research, applications. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  28. Juslin, P. N., & Vastfjall, D. (2008). Emotional responses to music: The need to consider underlying mechanisms. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 31(5), 559–621.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  29. Khalfa, S., Bruneau, N., Roge, B., Georgieff, N., Veuillet, E., Adrien, J. L., et al. (2004). Increased perception of loudness in autism. Hearing Research, 198(1–2), 87–92.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Khalfa, S., Isabelle, P., Jean-Pierre, B., & Manon, R. (2002). Event-related skin conductance responses to musical emotions in humans. Neuroscience Letters, 328(2), 145–149.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Kim, J., & Andre, E. (2008). Emotion recognition based on physiological changes in music listening. IEEE Transactions on Pattern Analysis and Machine Intelligence, 30(12), 2067–2083.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Koelsch, S. (2010). Towards a neural basis of music-evoked emotions. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 14(3), 131–137.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Konecni, V. J. (2005). The aesthetic trinity: Awe, being moved, thrills. Bulletin of Psychology and the Arts, 5, 27–44.Google Scholar
  34. Krumhansl, C. L. (1997). An exploratory study of musical emotions and psychophysiology. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology-Revue Canadienne De Psychologie Experimentale, 51(4), 336–353.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Levitin, D. (2006). This is your brain on music. New York: Dutton.Google Scholar
  36. Lombardo, M. V., Barnes, J. L., Wheelwright, S. J., & Baron-Cohen, S. (2007). Self-referential cognition and empathy in Autism. PLoS One, 2(9), 11.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Lutkenhoner, B., Seither-Preisler, A., & Seither, S. (2006). Piano tones evoke stronger magnetic fields than pure tones or noise, both in musicians and non-musicians. Neuroimage, 30(3), 927–937.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Main, I. G., Leonard, T., Papasouliotis, O., Hatton, C. G., & Meredith, P. G. (1999). One slope or two? Detecting statistically significant breaks of slope in geophysical data, with application to fracture scaling relationships. Geophysical Research Letters, 26(18), 2801–2804.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. McGregor, K. K., & Bean, A. (2012). How children with Autism extend new words. Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research, 55(1), 70–83.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Mottron, L., & Burack, J. A. (2001). Enhanced perceptual functioning in the development of autism. In J. A. Burack, T. Charman, N. Yirmiya, & P. R. Zelazo (Eds.), The development of autism: Perspectives from theory and research (pp. 131–148). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
  41. Mottron, L., Peretz, I., & Menard, E. (2000). Local and global processing of music in high-functioning persons with autism: Beyond central coherence? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 41(8), 1057–1065.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. North, A. C., Hargreaves, D. J., & Hargreaves, J. J. (2004). Uses of music in everyday life. Music Perception, 22(1), 41–77.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. North, A. C., Hargreaves, D. J., & O’Neill, S. A. (2000). The importance of music to adolescents. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 70, 255–272.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Peretz, I. (2001). Listen to the brain: A biological perspective on musical emotions. In P. Juslin & J. Sloboda (Eds.), Music and emotion. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  45. Plaisted, K. C. (2001). Reduced generalization in autism: An alternative to weak central coherence. In Development of Autism: Perspectives from theory and research (pp. 149–169). Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Assoc Publ.Google Scholar
  46. Quintin, E.-M., Bhatara, A., Poissant, H., Fombonne, E., & Levitin, D. J. (2011). Emotion perception in music in high-functioning adolescents with Autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 41(9), 1240–1255.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Salimpoor, V. N., Benovoy, M., Larcher, K., Dagher, A., & Zatorre, R. J. (2011). Anatomically distinct dopamine release during anticipation and experience of peak emotion to music. Nature Neuroscience, 14, 257–262.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Salimpoor, V. N., Benovoy, M., Longo, G., Cooperstock, J. R., & Zatorre, R. J. (2009). The rewarding aspects of music listening are related to degree of emotional arousal. PLoS One, 4(10), e7487.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Schaefer, F., & Boucsein, W. (2000). Comparison of electrodermal constant voltage and constant current recording techniques using the phase angle between alternating voltage and current. Psychophysiology, 37(1), 85–91.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Soulieres, I., Mottron, L., Giguere, G., & Larochelle, S. (2011). Category induction in autism: Slower, perhaps different, but certainly possible. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 64(2), 311–327.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Stewart, L. (2011). Characterizing congenital amusia. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 64(4), 625–638.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Vorst, H. C. M., & Bermond, B. (2001). Validity and reliability of the Bermond-Vorst Alexithymia Questionnaire. Personality and Individual Differences, 30(3), 413–434.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Zentner, M., Grandjean, D., & Scherer, K. R. (2008). Emotions evoked by the sound of music: Characterization, classification, and measurement. Emotion, 8(4), 494–521.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2012

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of PsychologyWhitehead Building, Goldsmiths, University of LondonLondonUK

Personalised recommendations