Advertisement

Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders

, Volume 43, Issue 1, pp 114–121 | Cite as

The Triple I Hypothesis: Taking Another(’s) Perspective on Executive Dysfunction in Autism

Original Paper

Abstract

The executive dysfunction theory attempts to explain not only the repetitive behaviours but also the socio-communicative difficulties in autism. While it is clear that some individuals with autism perform poorly on certain executive function tasks, it remains unclear what underlies these impairments. The most consistent and striking difficulties are seen on tasks that are open-ended in structure, lack explicit instructions and involve arbitrary rules. I propose that impairment on such tasks is not due to executive dysfunction; instead, poor performance results from difficulties forming an implicit understanding of the experimenter’s expectations for the task, resulting in egocentric and idiosyncratic behaviour. These difficulties in taking another’s perspective may be explained parsimoniously by the mentalising difficulties robustly demonstrated to exist in autism.

Keywords

Autism Executive function Theory of mind Mentalising Implicit Arbitrary 

Notes

Acknowledgments

The author wishes to thank John Morton and Uta Frith for helpful conversation and lively discussion whilst writing this paper and over a number of years. This work was supported by a British Academy Fellowship PDF/2009/213.

References

  1. Adams, N. C., & Jarrold, C. (2009). Inhibition and the validity of the stroop task for children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 39, 1112–1121.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Agam, Y., Joseph, R. M., Barton, J. J., & Manoach, D. S. (2010). Reduced cognitive control of response inhibition by the anterior cingulate cortex in autism spectrum disorders. Neuroimage, 52, 336–347.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Ambery, F. Z., Russell, A. J., Perry, K., Morris, R., & Murphy, D. G. (2006). Neuropsychological functioning in adults with Asperger syndrome. Autism, 10, 551–664.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Barnard, L., Muldoon, K., Hasan, R., O’Brien, G., & Stewart, M. (2008). Profiling executive dysfunction in adults with autism and comorbid learning disability. Autism, 12, 125–141.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Bigham, S. (2010). Impaired competence for pretense in children with autism: Exploring potential cognitive predictors. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 40, 30–38.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Bishop, D. V., & Norbury, C. F. (2005). Executive functions in children with communication impairments, in relation to autistic symptomatology. 2: Response inhibition. Autism, 9, 29–43.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Bogte, H., Flamma, B., van der Meere, J., & van Engeland, H. (2008). Cognitive flexibility in adults with high functioning autism. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 30, 33–41.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Boucher, J., Cowell, P., Howard, M., Broks, P., Farrant, A., Roberts, N., et al. (2005). A combined clinical, neuropsychological, and neuroanatomical study of adults with high functioning autism. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 10, 165–213.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Cederlund, M., Hagberg, B., & Gillberg, C. (2010). Asperger syndrome in adolescent and young adult males. Interview, self- and parent assessment of social, emotional, and cognitive problems. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 31, 287–298.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Chan, A. S., Cheung, M. C., Han, Y. M., Sze, S. L., Leung, W. W., Man, H. S., et al. (2009). Executive function deficits and neural discordance in children with autism spectrum disorders. Clinical Neurophysiology, 120, 1107–1115.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Colvert, E., Custance, D., & Swettenham, J. (2002). Rule-based reasoning and theory of mind in autism: A commentary on the work of Zelazo, Jacques, Burack and Frye. Infant and Child Development, 11, 197–200.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Corbett, B. A., Constantine, L. J., Hendren, R., Rocke, D., & Ozonoff, S. (2009). Examining executive functioning in children with autism spectrum disorder, attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and typical development. Psychiatry Research, 166, 210–222.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Dichter, G. S., & Belger, A. (2007). Social stimuli interfere with cognitive control in autism. Neuroimage, 35, 1219–1230.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Dichter, G. S., Radonovich, K. J., Turner-Brown, L. M., Lam, K. S., Holtzclaw, T. N., & Bodfish, J. W. (2010). Performance of children with autism spectrum disorders on the dimension-change card sort task. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 40, 448–456.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Edgin, J. O., & Pennington, B. F. (2005). Spatial cognition in autism spectrum disorders: superior, impaired, or just intact? Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 35, 729–745.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Fournier-Vicente, S., Larigauderie, P., & Gaonac’h, D. (2008). More dissociations and interactions within central executive functioning: A comprehensive latent-variable analysis. Acta Psychologia, 129, 32–48.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Gaigg, S. B., Gardiner, J. M., & Bowler, D. M. (2008). Free recall in autism spectrum disorder: The role of relational and item-specific encoding. Neuropsychologia, 46, 983–992.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Geurts, H. M., Corbett, B., & Solomon, M. (2009). The paradox of cognitive flexibility in autism. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13, 74–82.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Geurts, H. M., Verte, S., Oosterlaan, J., Roeyers, H., & Sergeant, J. A. (2004). How specific are executive functioning deficits in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and autism? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45, 836–854.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Gioia, G. A., Isquith, P. K., Kenworthy, L., & Barton, R. M. (2002). Profiles of everyday executive function in acquired and developmental disorders. Child Neuropsychology, 8, 121–137.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Goldberg, M. C., Mostofsky, S. H., Cutting, L. E., Mahone, E. M., Astor, B. C., Denckla, M. B., et al. (2005). Subtle executive impairment in children with autism and children with ADHD. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 35, 279–293.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Griebling, J., Minshew, N. J., Bodner, K., Libove, R., Bansal, R., Konasale, P., et al. (2010). Dorsolateral prefrontal cortex magnetic resonance imaging measurements and cognitive performance in autism. Journal of Child Neurology, 25, 856–863.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Hadjikhani, N., Joseph, R. M., Snyder, J., Chabris, C. F., Clark, J., Steele, S., et al. (2004). Activation of the fusiform gyrus when individuals with autism spectrum disorder view faces. Neuroimage, 22, 1141–1150.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Happé, F., Booth, R., Charlton, R., & Hughes, C. (2006). Executive function deficits in autism spectrum disorders and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: Examining profiles across domains and ages. Brain and Cognition, 61, 25–39.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Hill, E. L. (2004). Evaluating the theory of executive dysfunction in autism. Developmental Review, 24, 189–233.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Hill, E. L., & Bird, C. M. (2006). Executive processes in Asperger syndrome: Patterns of performance in a multiple case series. Neuropsychologia, 44, 2822–2835.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Joseph, R. M., McGrath, L. M., & Tager-Flusberg, H. (2005). Executive dysfunction and its relation to language ability in verbal school-age children with autism. Developmental Neuropsychology, 27, 361–378.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Joseph, R. M., & Tager-Flusberg, H. (2004). The relationship of theory of mind and executive functions to symptom type and severity in children with autism. Development and Psychopathology, 16, 137–155.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Kaland, N., Smith, L., & Mortensen, E. L. (2008). Brief report: Cognitive flexibility and focused attention in children and adolescents with Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism as measured on the computerized version of the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 38, 1161–1165.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Kalbfleisch, M.L., & Loughan, A.R., (2012). Impact of IQ discrepancy on executive function in high-functioning autism: Insight into Twice Exceptionality. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 42, 390–400.Google Scholar
  31. Kana, R. K., Keller, T. A., Minshew, N. J., & Just, M. A. (2007). Inhibitory control in high-functioning autism: Decreased activation and underconnectivity in inhibition networks. Biological Psychiatry, 62, 198–206.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Keary, C. J., Minshew, N. J., Bansal, R., Goradia, D., Fedorov, S., Keshavan, M. S., et al. (2009). Corpus callosum volume and neurocognition in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 39, 834–841.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Kenworthy, L., Yerys, B. E., Anthony, L. G., & Wallace, G. L. (2008). Understanding executive control in autism spectrum disorders in the lab and in the real world. Neuropsychology Review, 18, 320–338.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Kleinhans, N., Akshoomoff, N., & Delis, D. C. (2005). Executive functions in autism and Asperger’s disorder: Flexibility, fluency, and inhibition. Developmental Neuropsychology, 27, 379–401.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Landa, R. J., & Goldberg, M. C. (2005). Language, social, and executive functions in high functioning autism: a continuum of performance. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 35, 557–573.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Lee, P. S., Yerys, B. E., Della Rosa, A., Foss-Feig, J., Barnes, K. A., James, J. D., et al. (2009). Functional connectivity of the inferior frontal cortex changes with age in children with autism spectrum disorders: A fcMRI study of response inhibition. Cerebral Cortex, 19, 1787–1794.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Lopez, B. R., Lincoln, A. J., Ozonoff, S., & Lai, Z. (2005). Examining the relationship between executive functions and restricted, repetitive symptoms of autistic disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 35, 445–460.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Low, J., Goddard, E., & Melser, J. (2009). Generativity and imagination in autism spectrum disorder: Evidence from individual differences in children’s impossible entity drawings. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 27, 425–444.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Luna, B., Doll, S. K., Hegedus, S. J., Minshew, N. J., & Sweeney, J. A. (2007). Maturation of executive function in autism. Biological Psychiatry, 61, 474–481.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Mackinlay, R., Charman, T., & Karmiloff-Smith, A. (2006). High functioning children with autism spectrum disorder: A novel test of multitasking. Brain and Cognition, 61, 14–24.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., & Wager, T. D. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41, 49–100.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Morton, J., & Frith, U. (1995). Causal modeling: a structural approach to developmental psychopathology. In D. Cichetti & D. J. Cohen (Eds.), Manual of developmental psychopathology. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  43. Mosconi, M. W., Kay, M., D’Cruz, A. M., Seidenfeld, A., Guter, S., Stanford, L. D., et al. (2009). Impaired inhibitory control is associated with higher-order repetitive behaviors in autism spectrum disorders. Psychological Medicine, 39, 1559–1566.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Nakahachi, T., Iwase, M., Takahashi, H., Honaga, E., Sekiyama, R., Ukai, S., et al. (2006). Discrepancy of performance among working memory-related tasks in autism spectrum disorders was caused by task characteristics, apart from working memory, which could interfere with task execution. Psychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 60, 312–318.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Ozonoff, S. (1995). Reliability and validity of the wisconsin card sorting test in studies of autism. Neuropsychology, 9, 491–500.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Ozonoff, S., Cook, I., Coon, H., Dawson, G., Joseph, R. M., Klin, A., et al. (2004). Performance on Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated Battery subtests sensitive to frontal lobe function in people with autistic disorder: Evidence from the collaborative programs of excellence in autism network. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 34, 139–150.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Ozonoff, S., Pennington, B. F., & Rogers, S. J. (1991). Executive function deficits in high- functioning autistic individuals: relationship to theory of mind. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 32, 1081–1105.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Pellicano, E. (2007). Links between theory of mind and executive function in young children with autism: Clues to developmental primacy. Developmental Psychology, 43, 974–990.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Pellicano, E. (2010). The development of core cognitive skills in autism: A 3 year prospective study. Child Development, 81, 1400.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Pellicano, E., Maybery, M., Durkin, K., & Maley, A. (2006). Multiple cognitive capabilities/deficits in children with an autism spectrum disorder: “Weak” central coherence and its relationship to theory of mind and executive control. Development and Psychopathology, 18, 77–98.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Perner, J., & Lang, B. (1999). Development of theory of mind and executive control. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 3, 337–344.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Phelan, H. L., Filliter, J. H., & Johnson, S. A. (2010). Brief report: memory performance on the California verbal learning test–children’s version in Autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 41, 518–523.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Pierce, K., Müller, R. A., Ambrose, J., Allen, G., & Courchesne, E. (2001). Face processing occurs outside the fusiform ‘face area’ in autism: Evidence from functional MRI. Brain, 124, 2059–2073.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Poljac, E., Simon, S., Ringlever, L., Kalcik, D., Groen, W. B., Buitelaar, J. K., et al. (2010). Impaired task switching performance in children with dyslexia but not in children with autism. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 63, 401–416.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Robinson, S., Goddard, L., Dritschel, B., Wisley, M., & Howlin, P. (2009). Executive functions in children with autism spectrum disorders. Brain and Cognition, 71, 362–368.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. Russell, J. (1997). How executive disorders can bring about an inadequate “theory of mind”. In J. Russell (Ed.), Autism as an executive disorder. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  57. Russo, N., Flanagan, T., Iarocci, G., Berringer, D., Zelazo, P. D., & Burack, J. A. (2007). Deconstructing executive deficits among persons with autism: Implications for cognitive neuroscience. Brain and Cognition, 65, 77–86.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  58. Schmitz, N., Rubia, K., Daly, E., Smith, A., Williams, S., & Murphy, D. G. (2006). Neural correlates of executive function in autistic spectrum disorders. Biological Psychiatry, 59, 7–16.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  59. Semrud-Clikeman, M., Walkowiak, J., Wilkinson, A., & Butcher, B. (2010). Executive functioning in children with Asperger syndrome, ADHD-combined type, ADHD-predominately inattentive type, and controls. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 40, 1017–1027.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  60. Senju, A., Southgate, V., White, S. J., & Frith, U. (2009). Mindblind eyes: an absence of spontaneous theory of mind in Asperger syndrome. Science, 325, 883–885.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  61. Sinzig, J., Morsch, D., Bruning, N., Schmidt, M. H., & Lehmkuhl, G. (2008). Inhibition, flexibility, working memory and planning in autism spectrum disorders with and without comorbid ADHD-symptoms. Child and Adolescent Psychiatry and Mental Health, 2, 4.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  62. Towgood, K. J., Meuwese, J. D., Gilbert, S. J., Turner, M. S., & Burgess, P. W. (2009). Advantages of the multiple case series approach to the study of cognitive deficits in autism spectrum disorder. Neuropsychologia, 47, 2981–2988.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  63. Verte, S., Geurts, H. M., Roeyers, H., Oosterlaan, J., & Sergeant, J. A. (2005). Executive functioning in children with autism and Tourette syndrome. Development and Psychopathology, 17, 415–445.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  64. Voelbel, G. T., Bates, M. E., Buckman, J. F., Pandina, G., & Hendren, R. L. (2006). Caudate nucleus volume and cognitive performance: are they related in childhood psychopathology? Biological Psychiatry, 60, 942–950.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  65. White, S. J., Burgess, P. W., & Hill, E. L. (2009). Impairments on “open-ended” executive function tests in autism. Autism Research, 2, 138–147.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  66. Wilson, B. A., Alderman, N., Burgess, P. W., Emslie, H. C., & Evans, J. J. (1996). The behavioural assessment of the dysexecutive syndrome. Flempton, Bury St. EdmundsThames : Valley Test Company.Google Scholar
  67. Wimmer, H. (1989). Common-Sense Mentalismus und Emotion: entwicklungspsychologische Implikationen: I. In E. Roth (Ed.), Denken und Fühlen. Berlin: Springer-Verlag.Google Scholar
  68. Yerys, B. E., Hepburn, S. L., Pennington, B. F., & Rogers, S. J. (2007). Executive function in preschoolers with autism: Evidence consistent with a secondary deficit. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37, 1068–1079.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  69. Yerys, B. E., Wallace, G. L., Harrison, B., Celano, M. J., Giedd, J. N., & Kenworthy, L. E. (2009). Set-shifting in children with autism spectrum disorders: Reversal shifting deficits on the Intradimensional/Extradimensional Shift Test correlate with repetitive behaviors. Autism, 13, 523–538.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  70. Zinke, K., Fries, E., Altgassen, M., Kirschbaum, C., Dettenborn, L., & Kliegel, M. (2010). Visuospatial short-term memory explains deficits in tower task planning in high-functioning children with autism spectrum disorder. Child Neuropsychology, 16, 229–241.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2012

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Institute of Cognitive NeuroscienceUniversity College LondonLondonUK

Personalised recommendations