Advertisement

Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders

, Volume 42, Issue 10, pp 2141–2151 | Cite as

Do Adults with Autism Spectrum Disorders Compensate in Naturalistic Prospective Memory Tasks?

  • Mareike Altgassen
  • Nancy Koban
  • Matthias Kliegel
Original Paper

Abstract

The present study is the first to directly compare event- and time-based prospective memory in Autism Spectrum Disorders (ASD) using a contextual task mirroring real life demands of prospective memory. Twenty-five individuals with ASD and 25 age- and ability-matched controls completed the Dresden Breakfast task which required participants to prepare breakfast following a set of rules and time restrictions. Overall, adults with ASD had less correct time- and event-based prospective memory responses in comparison to controls, which is consistent with previous research in children with ASD. Moreover, ASD participants completed fewer tasks, followed rules less closely, and monitored the elapsing time less closely than controls. Individuals with ASD seem not to be compensating in naturalistic prospective memory tasks.

Keywords

Prospective memory Executive functions PDD Monitoring 

Notes

Acknowledgments

We are grateful to Professor Fergus Craik, Rotman Research Institute, Toronto, for his helpful comments on an earlier version of this manuscript. We would like to thank Pauline Greve, Anett Kretschmer and Sergio Cortez for their help in data collection and data analyses.

Supplementary material

10803_2012_1466_MOESM1_ESM.doc (24 kb)
Supplementary material 1 (DOC 23 kb)

References

  1. Altgassen, M., Schmitz-Hübsch, M., & Kliegel, M. (2010). Event-based prospective memory performance in autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Neurodevelopmental Disorders, 2, 2–8.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Altgassen, M., Williams, T. I., Bölte, S., & Kliegel, M. (2009). Time-based prospective memory in individuals with autism spectrum disorder. Brain Impairment, 10, 52–58.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. APA. (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders—Text revised (4th ed.). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.Google Scholar
  4. Arcara, G., Cona, G., Tarantino, V., & Bisiacchi, P. S. (2011). Neural correlates of prospective memory: A comparison between a time-based and an event-based task. Psychophysiology, 48, S75–S76.Google Scholar
  5. Bennetto, L., Pennington, B. F., & Rogers, S. J. (1996). Intact and impaired memory functions in autism. Child Development, 67, 1816–1835.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I., & Stefurak, T. (2008). Planning and task management in Parkinson’s disease: Differential emphasis in dual-task performance. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 14, 257–265.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Bölte, S., & Poustka, F. (2006). Fragebogen zur Sozialen Kommunikation—Autismus-Screening—FSK. Deutsche Fassung des Social Communication Questionnaire. Göttingen: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
  8. Bölte, S., Rühl, D., Schmötzer, G., & Poustka, F. (2006). Diagnostisches Interview für Autismus—Revidiert (ADI-R). Bern: Huber.Google Scholar
  9. Borys, S. V., Spitz, H. H., & Dorans, B. A. (1982). Tower of Hanoi performance of retarded young adults and nonretarded children as a function of solution length and goal state. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 33, 87–110.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Boucher, J., & Bowler, D. M. (Eds.). (2008). Memory in autism. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  11. Bowler, D. M., Gaigg, S. B., & Gardiner, J. M. (2008). Effects of related and unrelated context on recall and recognition by adults with high-functioning autism spectrum disorder. Neuropsychologia, 46, 993–999.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Bowler, D. M., Gardiner, J. M., & Berthollier, N. (2004). Source memory in adolescents and adults with Asperger’s syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 34, 533–542.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Bowler, D. M., Gardiner, J. M., & Grice, S. J. (2000a). Episodic memory and remembering in adults with Asperger syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 30, 295–304.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Bowler, D. M., Gardiner, J. M., Grice, S., & Saavalainen, P. (2000b). Memory illusions: False recall and recognition in adults with Asperger’s syndrome. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 109, 663–672.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Bowler, D. M., Matthews, N. J., & Gardiner, J. M. (1997). Asperger’s syndrome and memory: Similarity to autism but not amnesia. Neuropsychologia, 35, 65–70.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Brandimonte, M. A., Einstein, G. O., & McDaniel, M. A. (1996). Prospective memory: Theory and applications. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  17. Brandimonte, M. A., Filippello, P., Coluccia, E., Altgassen, M., & Kliegel, M. (2011). To do or not to do? Prospective memory versus response inhibition in autism spectrum disorder and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Memory, 19, 56–66.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Brunfaut, E., Vanoverberghe, V., & d’Ydewalle, G. (2000). Prospective remembering of Korsakoffs and alcoholics as a function of the prospective-memory and on-going tasks. Neuropsychologia, 38, 975–984.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Burgess, P. W., Gonen-Yaacovi, G., & Volle, E. (2011). Functional neuroimaging studies of prospective memory: What have we learnt so far? Neuropsychologia, 49, 2246–2257.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Burgess, P. W., Quayle, A., & Frith, C. D. (2001). Brain regions involved in prospective memory as determined by positron emission tomography. Neuropsychologia, 39, 545–555.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Burgess, P. W., Scott, S. K., & Frith, C. D. (2003). The role of the rostral frontal cortex (area 10) in prospective memory: A lateral versus medial dissociation. Neuropsychologia, 41, 906–918.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Craik, F. I. M., & Bialystok, E. (2006). Planning and task management in older adults: Cooking breakfast. Memory & Cognition, 34, 1236–1249.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Dawson, M., Soulieres, I., Gernsbacher, M. A., & Mottron, L. (2007). The level and nature of autistic intelligence. Psychological Science, 18, 657–662.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Dobbs, A. R., & Rule, B. G. (1987). Prospective memory and self-reports of memory abilities in older adults. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 41, 209–222.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Einstein, G. O., & McDaniel, M. A. (1996). Retrieval processes in prospective memory: Theoretical approaches and some new findings. In M. A. Brandimonte, G. O. Einstein, & M. A. McDaniel (Eds.), Prospective memory: Theory and applications (pp. 115–142). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  26. Einstein, G. O., & McDaniel, M. A. (2005). Prospective memory: Multiple retrieval processes. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 14, 286–290.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Ellis, J. (1996). Prospective memory or the realization of delayed intentions: A conceptual framework for research. In M. A. Brandimonte, G. O. Einstein, & M. A. McDaniel (Eds.), Prospective memory: Theory and applications (pp. 1–22). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  28. Farrant, A., Blades, M., & Boucher, J. (1998). Source monitoring by children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 28, 43–50.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Hala, S., Rasmussen, C., & Henderson, A. M. E. (2005). Three types of source monitoring by children with and without autism: The role of executive function. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 35, 75–89.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Häußler, A., Happel, C., Tuckermann, A., Altgassen, M., & Adl-Amini, K. (2003). SOKO Autismus: Gruppenangebote zur Förderung SOzialer KOmpetenzen bei Menschen mit Autismus Erfahrungsbericht und Praxishilfen. Dortmund: Verlag Modernes Lernen.Google Scholar
  31. Henry, J. D., MacLeod, M. S., Phillips, L. H., & Crawford, J. R. (2004). A meta-analytic review of prospective memory and aging. Psychology and Aging, 19, 27–39.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Henry, J. D., Rendell, P. G., Kliegel, M., & Altgassen, M. (2007). Prospective memory in schizophrenia: Primary or secondary impairment? Schizophrenia Research, 95, 179–185.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Hill, E. L. (2004). Evaluating the theory of executive dysfunction in autism. Developmental Review, 24, 189–233.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Hoppe, C., Müller, U., Werheid, K., Thöne, A., & von Cramon, D. Y. (2000). Digit ordering test: Clinical, psychometric, and experimental evaluation of a verbal working memory test. Clinical Neuropsychologist, 14, 38–55.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  35. Hughes, C. (1996). Brief report: Planning problems in autism at the level of motor control. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 26, 99–107.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Hughes, C., Russell, J., & Robbins, T. W. (1994). Evidence for executive dysfunction in autism. Neuropsychologia, 32, 477–492.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Kliegel, M., Altgassen, M., Hering, A., & Rose, N. S. (2011). A process-model based approach to prospective memory impairment in Parkinson’s disease. Neuropsychologia, 49, 2166–2177.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Kliegel, M., Eschen, A., & Thöne-Otto, A. I. T. (2004). Planning and realization of complex intentions in traumatic brain injury and normal aging. Brain and Cognition, 56, 43–54.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Kliegel, M., Jäger, T., Altgassen, M., & Shum, D. (2008a). Clinical neuropsychology of prospective memory. In M. Kliegel, M. A. McDaniel, & G. O. Einstein (Eds.), Prospective memory: Cognitive, neuroscience, developmental, and applied perspectives (pp. 283–308). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  40. Kliegel, M., Martin, M., McDaniel, M. A., & Einstein, G. O. (2002). Complex prospective memory and executive control of working memory: A process model. Psychology Science, 44, 303–318.Google Scholar
  41. Kliegel, M., Martin, M., McDaniel, M. A., & Phillips, L. H. (2007). Adult age differences in errand planning: The role of task familiarity and cognitive resources. Experimental Aging Research, 33, 145–161.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Kliegel, M., McDaniel, M. A., & Einstein, G. O. (2008b). Prospective memory: Cognitive, neuroscience, developmental, and applied perspectives. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  43. Lord, C., Risi, S., Lambrecht, L., Cook, E. H., Leventhal, B. L., DiLavore, P. C., et al. (2000). The autism diagnostic observation schedule-generic: A standard measure of social and communication deficits associated with the spectrum of autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 30, 205–223.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Lord, C., Rutter, M., & Lecouteur, A. (1994). Autism diagnostic interview-revised—A revised version of a diagnostic interview for caregivers of individuals with possible pervasive developmental disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 24, 659–685.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Loth, E., Gomez, J. C., & Happe, F. (2011). Do high-functioning people with autism spectrum disorder spontaneously use event knowledge to selectively attend to and remember context-relevant aspects in scenes? Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 41, 945–961.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Mackinlay, R. J., Charman, T., & Karmiloff-Smith, A. (2006). High-functioning children with autism spectrum disorder: A novel test of multitasking. Brain and Cognition, 61, 14–24.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. McDaniel, M. A., & Einstein, G. O. (2000). Strategic and automatic processes in prospective memory retrieval: A multiprocess framework. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 14, S127–S144.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. McDaniel, M. A., Glisky, E. L., Rubin, S. R., Guynn, M. J., & Routhieaux, B. C. (1999). Prospective memory: A neuropsychological study. Neuropsychology, 13, 103–110.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Minshew, N. J., & Goldstein, G. (2001). The pattern of intact and impaired memory functions in autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 42, 1095–1101.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., & Wager, T. D. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41, 49–100.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. O’Shea, A. G., Fein, D. A., Cillessen, A. H. N., Klin, A., & Schultz, R. T. (2005). Source memory in children with autism spectrum disorders. Developmental Neuropsychology, 27, 337–360.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Ozonoff, S., Dawson, G., & McPartland, J. (2002). A parent’s guide to Asperger syndrome and high functioning autism: How to meet the challenges and help your child thrive. New York: The Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  53. Ozonoff, S., & Jensen, J. (1999). Specific executive function profiles in three neurodevelopmental disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 29, 171–177.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Ozonoff, S., & Strayer, D. L. (1997). Inhibitory function in nonretarded children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 27, 59–77.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Ozonoff, S., Strayer, D. L., McMahon, W. M., & Filloux, F. (1994). Executive function abilities in autism and Tourette syndrome—An information-processing approach. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 35, 1015–1032.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. Prior, M. R., & Hoffmann, W. (1990). Neuropsychological testing of autistic-children through an exploration with Frontal-Lobe tests. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 20, 581–590.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  57. Rendell, P. G., & Thomson, D. M. (1999). Aging and prospective memory: Differences between naturalistic and laboratory tasks. The Journals of Gerontology. Series B, Psychological sciences and social sciences, 54, 256–269.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  58. Renner, P., Klinger, L. G., & Klinger, M. (2000). Implicit and explicit memory in autism: Is autism an amnesic disorder? Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 30, 3–14.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  59. Rühl, D., Bölte, S., Feineis-Matthews, S., & Poustka, F. (2004). Diagnostische Beobachtungsskala für Autistische Störungen (ADOS). Bern: Huber.Google Scholar
  60. Rumsey, J. M., & Hamburger, S. D. (1988). Neuropsychological findings in high-functioning men with infantile autism, residual state. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 10, 201–221.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  61. Russell, J., & Jarrold, C. (1999). Memory for actions in children with autism: Self versus other. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 4, 303–331.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  62. Rutter, M., Bailey, A., & Lord, C. (2003). Social communication questionnaire. Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services.Google Scholar
  63. Salthouse, T. A. (1984). Effects of age and skill in typing. Journal of Experimental Psychology-General, 113, 345–371.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  64. Schopler, E., Mesibov, G. B., & Hearsey, K. (1995). Structured teaching in the TEACCH system. In E. Schopler & G. B. Mesibov (Eds.), Learning and cognition in autism (Vol 243–267). New York: Plenum Press.Google Scholar
  65. Steiger, J. H. (1980). Tests for comparing elements of a correlation matrix. Psychological Bulletin, 87, 245–251.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  66. Tager-Flusberg, H. (1991). Semantic processing in the free recall of autistic children: Further evidence for a cognitive deficit. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 9, 417–430.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  67. Wechsler, D. (1999). Wechsler abbreviated scale of intelligence—WASI. Oxford: Pearson.Google Scholar
  68. West, R. (2008). The cognitive neuroscience of prospective memory. In M. Kliegel, M. A. McDaniel, & G. O. Einstein (Eds.), Prospective memory: Cognitive, neuroscience, developmental, and applied perspectives (pp. 261–282). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  69. WHO. (2006). International statistical classification of diseases and related health problems: 10th revision. Geneva: World Health Organization.Google Scholar
  70. Wilson, B. A., Alderman, N., Burgess, P. W., Emslie, H., & Evans, J. J. (1996). Behavioural assessment of the dysexecutive syndrome BADS. Bury St. Edmunds: Thames Valley Test Company.Google Scholar
  71. Zeintl, M., Kliegel, M., & Hofer, S. M. (2007). The role of processing resources in age-related prospective and retrospective memory within old age. Psychology and Aging, 22, 826–834.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  72. Zimmermann, P. & Fimm, B. (2002). Testbatterie zur Aufmerksamkeitsprüfung, Version 1.7. Herzogenrath: Vera Fimm.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2012

Authors and Affiliations

  • Mareike Altgassen
    • 1
  • Nancy Koban
    • 1
  • Matthias Kliegel
    • 1
    • 2
  1. 1.Department of PsychologyTechnische Universitaet DresdenDresdenGermany
  2. 2.Department of PsychologyUniversity of GenevaGenevaSwitzerland

Personalised recommendations