Advertisement

Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders

, Volume 42, Issue 8, pp 1557–1565 | Cite as

The Big Picture: Storytelling Ability in Adults with Autism Spectrum Conditions

  • Jennifer L. BarnesEmail author
  • Simon Baron-Cohen
Original Paper

Abstract

Previous work on story-telling ability in autism spectrum conditions (ASC) has found a pattern of relatively intact use of story grammar in ASC narratives; however, prior analysis has concentrated primarily on whether specific story components are included, rather than how they are included. The present study analyzes an existing narrative dataset, concentrating on the kind of information that individuals with and without high functioning autism or Asperger syndrome include about story elements such as setting, character, conflict, and resolution. This analysis showed that individuals with ASC are biased toward providing local over global details about each element, regardless of whether the element involved mental content. These results are discussed in terms of the Weak Central Coherence and Hyper-Systemizing theories.

Keywords

Autism Story-telling Narrative Verbal ability Central coherence Film 

Notes

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by a Fulbright Fellowship to JB and by funding from the MRC to SBC. The authors would like to thank Sally Wheelwright and Michael Lombardo for their help on the original study, from which this paper draws its data. A version of this paper was presented at the Biennial Meeting of the Society for Research on Child Development. Stimuli were taken from the following episodes in the first two seasons of the television show House: “Sports Medicine” © 2005 Universal Network Television LLC; “Histories” © 2005 Universal Network Television LLC; “Autopsy” © 2005 Universal Network Television LLC; “Need To Know” © 2006 MORATIM Produktions GmbH & Co. KG. Clips furnished through the courtesy of Universal Network Television LLC. We are grateful to Ilona Roth and Johnny Lawson for valuable discussions, to Michael Lombardo for his help gathering data, and to the participants for their generous cooperation.

References

  1. APA. (1994). DSM-IV diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed.). Washington DC: American Psychiatric Association.Google Scholar
  2. Barnes, J., Lombardo, M., Wheelwright, S., & Baron-Cohen, S. (2009). Moral dilemmas film task: A study of spontaneous narratives by individuals with autism spectrum conditions. Autism Research, 2(3), 148–156.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Baron-Cohen, S (2006). The hyper-systemizing, assortative mating theory of autism. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry, 30(5), 865–872.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Baron-Cohen, S. et al. (2001). The Autism-Spectrum Quotient (AQ): Evidence from asperger syndrome, high functioning autism, males and females, and scientists and mathematicians. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 31(1), 5–17.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A. M., & Frith, U. (1985). Does the autistic child have a “theory of mind”? Cognition, 21(1), 37–46.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Baron-Cohen, S., Richler, J., Bisarya, D., Gurunathan, N., & Wheelwright, S. (2003). The systemizing quotient: An investigation of adults with Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism, and normal sex differences. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, Series B, Special issue on Autism: Mind and Brain, 358, 361–374.Google Scholar
  7. Beaumont, R., & Newcombe, P. (2006). Theory of mind and central coherence in adults with high-functioning autism or Asperger syndrome. Autism, 10(4), 365–382.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Bennetto, L., & Pennington, B. F. (2003). Executive functioning in normal and abnormal development. In S. Segalowitz & I. Rapin (Eds.), Child neuropsychology, part II (2nd ed., Vol. 8). Amsterdam: Elsevier.Google Scholar
  9. Berman, A., & Slobin, B. (1994). Relating events in narrative: A crosslinguistic study. Hillsdale, New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.Google Scholar
  10. Booth, R., Charlton, R., Hughes, C., & Happe′, F. (2003). Disentangling weak coherence and executive dysfunction: Planning drawing in autism and ADHD. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, 358(1430), 387–392.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Bruner, J., & Feldman, C. (1993). Theories of mind and the problem of autism. In S. Baron-Cohen, H. Tager-Flusberg, & D. J. Cohen (Eds.), Understanding other minds: Perspectives from autism (pp. 267–291). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  12. Capps, L., Losh, M., & Thurber, C. (2000). “The frog ate the bug and made his mouth sad”: Narrative competence in children with autism. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 28(2), 193–204.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Colle, L., Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelright, S., & van der Lely, H. (2008). Narrative discourse in adults with high-functioning autism or Asperger syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 38(1), 28–40.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Diehl, J. J., Bennetto, L., & Young, E. C. (2006). Story recall and narrative coherence of high-functioning children with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 34(1), 87–102.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Frith, U. (1989). Autism: Explaining the enigma. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
  16. Goldman, S. (2008). Brief report: Narratives of personal events in children with autism and developmental language disorders: Unshared memories. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 38, 1982–1988.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Happé, F., & Frith, U. (2006). The weak coherence account: Detail-focused cognitive style in autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36(1), 5–24.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Jolliffe, T., & Baron-Cohen, S. (1997). Are people with autism or Asperger’s syndrome faster than normal on the embedded figures task? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 38, 527–534.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Klin, A. (2000). Attributing social meaning to ambiguous visual stimuli in higher-functioning autism and Asperger syndrome: The social attribution task. Journal of Child Psychology, 41(7), 831–846.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Losh, M., & Capps, L. (2003). Narrative ability in high-functioning children with autism or Asperger’s syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 33, 239–251.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Loveland, K. A. (1989). Narrative story-telling in autism and Down syndrome. Paper presented at the Society for Research in Child Development, Kansas City, MO, April 27–30.Google Scholar
  22. Mar, R. A. (2004). The neuropsychology of narrative: story comprehension, story production and their interrelation. Neuropsychologia, 42(10), 1414–1434.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Mar, R., & Oatley, K. (2008). The function of fiction is the abstraction and simulation of social experience. Perspectives on psychological science, 3(3), 173–192.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Mar, R. A., Oatley, K., Hirsh, J., de la Paz, J., & Peterson, J. B. (2006). Bookworms versus nerds: Exposure to fiction versus non-fiction, divergent associations with social ability, and the simulation of fictional worlds. Journal of Research in Personality, 40, 694–712.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Norbury, C., & Bishop, D. V. M. (2003). Narrative skills of children with communication impairments. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 3, 287–313.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Oatley, K. (1999). Why fiction may be twice as true as fact: Fiction as cognitive and emotional simulation. Review of General Psychology, 3, 101–117.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. O’Riordan, M. A., Plaisted, K. C., Driver, J., & Baron-Cohen, S. (2001). Superior visual search in autism. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27(3), 719–730.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Shah, A., & Frith, U. (1983). An islet of ability in autism: A research note. The Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 24, 613–620.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Shah, A., & Frith, U. (1993). Why do autistic individuals show superior performance on the block design task? The Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 34(8), 1351–1364.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Sperber, D., & Wilson, D. (1986). Relevance: Communication and cognition. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
  31. Strong, C. J. (1998). The strong narrative assessment procedure. Madison, WI: Thinking Publications.Google Scholar
  32. Sugiyama, M. (1996). On the origins of narrative: Storyteller bias as a fitness-enhancing strategy. Human Nature, 7, 403–425.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Wechsler, D. (1999). Wechsler abbreviated scale of intelligence. San Antonio: Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
  34. Young, E. C., Diehl, J. J., Morries, D., Hyman, S., & Bennetto, L. (2005). The use of two language tests to identify pragmatic language problems in children with autism spectrum disorders. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 36, 62–72.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2011

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Autism Research Centre, Department of PsychiatryCambridge UniversityCambridgeUK
  2. 2.Department of PsychologyYale UniversityNew HavenUSA
  3. 3.New HavenUSA

Personalised recommendations