Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders

, Volume 41, Issue 8, pp 1097–1112 | Cite as

Processing of Ironic Language in Children with High-Functioning Autism Spectrum Disorder

  • Penny M. Pexman
  • Kristin R. Rostad
  • Carly A. McMorris
  • Emma A. Climie
  • Jacqueline Stowkowy
  • Melanie R. Glenwright
Original Paper

Abstract

We examined processing of verbal irony in three groups of children: (1) 18 children with high-functioning Autism Spectrum Disorder (HFASD), (2) 18 typically-developing children, matched to the first group for verbal ability, and (3) 18 typically-developing children matched to the first group for chronological age. We utilized an irony comprehension task that minimized verbal and pragmatic demands for participants. Results showed that children with HFASD were as accurate as typically-developing children in judging speaker intent for ironic criticisms, but group differences in judgment latencies, eye gaze, and humor evaluations suggested that children with HFASD applied a different processing strategy for irony comprehension; one that resulted in less accurate appreciation of the social functions of irony.

Keywords

Verbal irony High-functioning Autism spectrum disorder Language processing Eye gaze Figurative language 

References

  1. Ackerman, B. P. (1983). Form and function in children’s understanding of ironic utterances. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 35, 487–508.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Adachi, T., Koeda, T., Hirabayashi, S., Maeoka, Y., Shiota, M., Wright, E. C., et al. (2004). The metaphor and sarcasm scenario test: A new instrument to help differentiate high functioning pervasive developmental disorder from attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Brain and Development, 26, 301–306.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Bauminger, N., Solomon, M., Aviezer, A., Heung, K., Gazit, L., Brown, J., et al. (2008). Children with autism and their friends: A multidimensional study of friendship in high-functioning autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 36, 135–150.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Bishop, D. V. M. (1998). Development of the Children’s Communication Checklist (CCC): A method for assessing qualitative aspects of communicative impairment in children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 39, 879–891.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Climie, E. A., & Pexman, P. M. (2008). Eye gaze provides a window on children’s understanding of verbal irony. Journal of Cognition & Development, 9, 257–285.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Colston, H. L. (1997). Salting a wound or sugaring a pill: The pragmatic functions of ironic criticisms. Discourse Processes, 23, 24–45.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Dennis, M., Lazenby, A. L., & Lockyer, L. (2001). Inferential language in high-function children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 31, 47–54.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Dews, S., & Winner, E. (1995). Muting the meaning: A social function of irony. Metaphor & Symbolic Activity, 10, 3–19.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Dews, S., Winner, E., Kaplan, J., Rosenblatt, E., Hunt, M., Lim, K., et al. (1996). Children’s understanding of the meaning and functions of verbal irony. Child Development, 67, 3071–3085.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Filippova, E., & Astington, J. W. (2008). Further development in social reasoning revealed in discourse irony understanding. Child Development, 79, 126–138.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Gibbs, R. (1994). The poetics of mind: Figurative thought, language, and understanding. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  12. Grice, H. P. (1975). Logic and conversation. In P. Cole & J. L. Morgan (Eds.), Syntax and semantics: Speech acts (Vol. 3, pp. 41–58). New York: Academic.Google Scholar
  13. Guerts, H. M., Verté, S., Oosterlaan, J., Roeyers, H., Hartman, C. A., Mulder, E. J., et al. (2004). Can the Children’s Communication Checklist differentiate between children with autism, children with ADHD, and normal controls? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45, 1437–1453.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Happé, F. G. E. (1993). Communicative competence and theory of mind in autism: A test of relevance theory. Cognition, 48, 101–119.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Harris, M., & Pexman, P. M. (2003). Children’s perceptions of the social functions of verbal irony. Discourse Processes, 36, 147–165.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Jarvinen-Pasley, A., Pasley, J., & Heaton, P. (2008). Is the linguistic content of speech less salient than its perceptual features in autism? Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 38, 239–248.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Just, M. A., Cherkassky, V. L., Keller, T. A., & Minshew, N. J. (2004). Cortical activation and synchronization during sentence comprehension in high-functioning autism: Evidence of underconnectivity. Brain, 127, 1811–1821.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Kail, R. (1991). Developmental change in speed of processing during childhood and adolescence. Psychological Bulletin, 109, 490–501.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Kail, R., & Salthouse, T. A. (1994). Processing speed as a mental capacity. Acta Psychologica, 86, 199–225.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Kaland, N., Moller-Nielsen, A., Callesen, K., Mortensen, E. L., Gottlieb, D., & Smith, L. (2002). A new ‘advanced’ test of theory of mind: Evidence from children and adolescents with Asperger syndrome. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 43, 517–528.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Kasari, C., Chamberlain, B., & Bauminger, N. (2001). Social emotions and social relationships: Can children with autism compensate? In J. Burack, T. Charman, N. Yirmiya, & P. Zelazo (Eds.), The development of autism: Perspectives from theory and research (pp. 309–323). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  22. Lord, C., & Paul, R. (1997). Language and communication in autism. In D. J. Cohen & F. R. Volkmar (Eds.), Handbook of autism and pervasive developmental disorders (2nd ed., pp. 195–225). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  23. Losh, M., & Capps, L. (2006). Understanding of emotional experience in autism: Insights from the personal accounts of high-functioning children with autism. Developmental Psychology, 42, 809–818.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. MacKay, G., & Shaw, A. (2004). A comparative study of figurative language in children with autistic spectrum disorders. Child Language Teaching and Therapy, 20, 13–32.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Martin, I., & McDonald, S. (2004). An exploration of causes of non-literal language problems in individuals with Asperger syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 34, 311–328.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Newcomer, P. L., & Hammill, D. D. (1997). Test of language development, primary (3rd ed.). Austin, TX: Pro-ed.Google Scholar
  27. Nilsen, E. S., Graham, S. A., Smith, S. L., & Chambers, C. G. (2008). Preschoolers’ sensitivity to referential ambiguity: Evidence for a dissociation between implicit understanding and explicit behavior. Developmental Science, 11, 556–562.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. O’Connor, K., & Kirk, I. (2008). Brief Report: Atypical social cognition and social behaviors in autism spectrum disorder: A different way of processing rather than an impairment. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 38, 1989–1997.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Paul, R., Orlovski, S. M., Marcinko, H. C., & Volkmar, F. (2009). Conversational behaviors in youth with high-functioning ASD and Asperger Syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 39, 115–125.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Pexman, P. M. (2008). It’s fascinating research: The cognition of verbal irony. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 17, 287–290.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Pexman, P. M., & Glenwright, M. (2007). How do typically-developing children grasp the meaning of verbal irony? Journal of Neurolinguistics, 20, 178–196.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Pexman, P. M., & Olineck, K. M. (2002). Does sarcasm always sting? Investigating the impact of ironic insults and ironic compliments. Discourse Processes, 33, 199–217.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Solomon, M., Goodlin-Jones, B. L., & Anders, T. F. (2004). A social adjustment enhancement intervention for high functioning autism, asperger’s syndrome, and pervasive developmental disorder NOS. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 34, 649–668.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Tager-Flusberg, H., Paul, R., & Lord, C. (2005). Language and Communication in Autism. In F. R. Volkmar, R. Paul, A. Klin, & D. Cohen (Eds.), Handbook of Autism and Pervasive Developmental Disorders (3rd ed., pp. 335–364). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
  35. Trueswell, J., & Gleitman, L. (2004). Children’s eye movements during listening: Developmental evidence for a constraint-based theory of sentence processing. In J. M. Henderson & F. Ferreira (Eds.), The interface of language, vision, and action: Eye movements and the visual world (pp. 319–346). New York, NY: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
  36. Wallace, G. L., Anderson, M., & Happé, F. (2009). Brief report: Information processing speed is intact in autism but not correlated with measured intelligence. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 39, 809–814.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Wang, A. T., Lee, S. S., Sigman, M., & Dapretto, M. (2006). Neural basis of irony comprehension in children with autism: The role of prosody and context. Brain, 129, 932–943.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Whalen, J. M., & Pexman, P. M. (2010). How do children respond to verbal irony in face-to-face communication? The development of mode adoption across middle childhood. Discourse Processes, 47, 363–387.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2010

Authors and Affiliations

  • Penny M. Pexman
    • 1
  • Kristin R. Rostad
    • 1
  • Carly A. McMorris
    • 1
    • 4
  • Emma A. Climie
    • 2
  • Jacqueline Stowkowy
    • 1
  • Melanie R. Glenwright
    • 3
  1. 1.Department of PsychologyUniversity of CalgaryCalgaryCanada
  2. 2.Division of Applied PsychologyUniversity of CalgaryCalgaryCanada
  3. 3.Department of PsychologyUniversity of ManitobaWinnipegCanada
  4. 4.Department of PsychologyYork UniversityTorontoCanada

Personalised recommendations