Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders

, Volume 40, Issue 11, pp 1403–1411 | Cite as

A Comparison of the Development of Audiovisual Integration in Children with Autism Spectrum Disorders and Typically Developing Children

Original Paper

Abstract

This study aimed to investigate the development of audiovisual integration in children with Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD). Audiovisual integration was measured using the McGurk effect in children with ASD aged 7–16 years and typically developing children (control group) matched approximately for age, sex, nonverbal ability and verbal ability. Results showed that the children with ASD were delayed in visual accuracy and audiovisual integration compared to the control group. However, in the audiovisual integration measure, children with ASD appeared to ‘catch-up’ with their typically developing peers at the older age ranges. The suggestion that children with ASD show a deficit in audiovisual integration which diminishes with age has clinical implications for those assessing and treating these children.

Keywords

Autism Spectrum Disorder Audiovisual integration Development 

References

  1. APA. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (revised) (4th ed.). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.Google Scholar
  2. Bebko, J., Weiss, J., Demark, J., & Gomez, P. (2006). Discrimination of temporal synchrony in intermodal events by children with autism and children with developmental disabilities without autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 47, 88–98.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  3. Bohning, M., Campbell, R., & Karmiloff-Smith, A. (2002). Audiovisual speech perception in Williams Syndrome. Neuropsychologia, 40, 1396–1406.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  4. Burack, J. A., Iarocci, G., Flanagan, T. D., & Bowler, D. M. (2004). On mosaics and melting pots: Conceptual considerations of comparison and matching strategies. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 34(1), 65–73.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  5. Calvert, G. A., Brammer, M. J., & Iverson, S. D. (1998). Crossmodal identification. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 2, 247–253.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Calvert, G. A., & Campbell, R. (2003). Reading speech from still and moving faces: The neural substrates of visible speech. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 15(1), 57–70.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  7. de Gelder, B., Vroomen, J., & van der Heide, L. (1991). Face recognition and lip-reading in autism. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 3(1), 69–86.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Dunn, L. M., Dunn, L. M., Whetton, C., & Burley, J. (1997). The British picture vocabulary scale (2nd ed.). London: NFER-Nelson.Google Scholar
  9. Dupont, S., Aubin, J., & Menard, L. (2005). A study of the McGurk effect in 4 and 5-year-old French Canadian children. ZAS Papers in Linguistics, 40, 1–17.Google Scholar
  10. Hockley, N. S., & Polka, L. (1994). A developmental study of audiovisual speech perception using the McGurk paradigm. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 96(5), 3309.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Iarocci, G., & McDonald, J. (2006). Sensory integration and the perceptual experience of persons with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36(1), 77–90.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  12. Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1998). Development itself is the key to understanding developmental disorders. Trends in Cognitive Science, 2(10), 389–398.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Karmiloff-Smith, A., Thomas, M., Annaz, D., Humphreys, K., Ewing, S., Brace, N., et al. (2004). Exploring the Williams Syndrome face-processing debate: The importance of developmental trajectories. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45, 1–17.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Kilner, J. M., & Blakemore, S. (2007). How does the mirror neuron system change during development? Developmental Science, 10(5), 524–526.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  15. Massaro, D. (1987). Speech perception by ear and by eye: A paradigm for psychological inquiry. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  16. McGurk, H., & MacDonald, J. (1976). Hearing lips and seeing voices. Nature, 264, 746–748.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  17. Mongillo, E. A., Irwin, J. R., Whalen, D. H., Klaiman, C., Carter, A. S., & Schultz, R. T. (2008). Audiovisual processing in children with and without autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 38, 1349–1358.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  18. Mottron, L. (2004). Matching strategies in cognitive research with individuals with high-functioning autism: Current practices, instrument biases, and recommendations. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 34(1), 19–27.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  19. Raven, J., Raven, J. C., & Court, J. H. (1998). Standard progressive matrices. Oxford: Oxford Psychologists Press.Google Scholar
  20. Rippon, G., Brock, J., Brown, C., & Boucher, J. (2007). Disordered connectivity in the autistic brain: Challenges for the ‘new psychophysiology’. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 63, 164–172.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  21. Rutter, M., Bailey, A., Berument, S. K., Lord, C., & Pickles, A. (2003). Social communication questionnaire (SCQ). Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services.Google Scholar
  22. Schopler, E., Reichler, R. J., & Rochen Renner, B. (2002). The childhood autism rating scale (CARS). Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services.Google Scholar
  23. Sekiyama, K., & Burnham, D. (2008). Impact of language on development of auditory-visual speech perception. Developmental Science, 11(2), 306–320.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  24. Smith, E., & Bennetto, L. (2007). Audiovisual speech integration and lipreading in autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 48(8), 813–821.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  25. Tabachnick, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (1996). Using multivariate statistics (3rd ed.). New York: Harper Collins.Google Scholar
  26. Thomas, M. S. C., Annaz, D., Ansari, D., Scerif, G., Jarrold, C., & Karmiloff-Smith, A. (2009). Using developmental trajectories to understand developmental disorders. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 52, 336.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Tremblay, C., Champoux, F., Voss, P., Bacon, B., Lepore, F., & Théoret, H. (2007). Speech and non-speech audio-visual illusions: A developmental study. PLoS ONE, 2(8), e742. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0000742.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  28. van der Smagt, M., van Engeland, H., & Kemner, C. (2007). Brief report: Can you see what is not there? Low-level auditory-visual integration in autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37, 2014–2019.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  29. Williams, J., Massaro, D., Peel, N., Bosseler, A., & Suddendorf, T. (2004). Visual-auditory integration during speech imitation in autism. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 25, 559–575.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  30. Williams, J., Whiten, A., Suddendorf, T., & Perrett, D. (2001). Imitation, mirror neurons and autism. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Review, 25, 287–295.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2010

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Clinical Psychology UnitUniversity of SheffieldSheffield, South YorkshireUK
  2. 2.Department of PsychologyUniversity of SheffieldSheffield, South YorkshireUK

Personalised recommendations