Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders

, Volume 39, Issue 8, pp 1211–1217 | Cite as

Brief Report: Attenuated Emotional Suppression of the Attentional Blink in Autism Spectrum Disorder: Another Non-Social Abnormality?

Brief Report

Abstract

Twenty-five individuals with Autism Spectrum Disorder and 25 typically developed individuals participated in an Attentional Blink paradigm to determine whether emotional words would capture attention similarly in the two groups. Whilst the emotionality of words facilitated attention in typical comparison participants, this effect was attenuated in the ASD group. The magnitude of the emotional modulation of attention in ASD also correlated significantly with participants’ VIQ, which was not observed for the comparison group. Together these observations replicate and extend the findings of Corden et al. (J Autism Develop Disord 38:1072–1080, 2008) and implicate abnormalities in emotional processes outside the broader context of social cognition in ASD. We discuss our findings in relation to possible abnormalities in amygdala function that may underlie the disorder.

Keywords

Emotional modulation of attention Attentional Blink Autism Spectrum Disorder Emotional processing Amygdala 

References

  1. American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (DSM-IV-TR). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Anderson, A. K., & Phelps, E. A. (2001). Lesions of the human amygdala impair enhanced perception of emotionally salient events. Nature, 411, 305–309. doi:10.1038/35077083.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Armony, J. L., & Dolan, R. J. (2001). Modulation of spatial attention by fear-conditioned stimuli: An event related fMRI study. Neuropsychologia, 1322, 1–10.Google Scholar
  4. Ashwin, C., Wheelwright, S., & Baron-Cohen, S. (2006). Finding a face in the crowd: Testing the anger superiority effect in Asperger Syndrome. Brain and Cognition, 61, 78–95. doi:10.1016/j.bandc.2005.12.008.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Bachevalier, J. (2000). The amygdala, social behaviour, and autism. In J. B. Aggleton (Ed.), The amygdala (2nd ed., pp. 509–543). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  6. Baron-Cohen, S. (1995). Mindblindness: An essay on autism and theory of mind. Cambridge: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  7. Baron-Cohen, S., Ring, H. A., Bullmore, E. T., Wheelwright, S., Ashwin, C., & Williams, S. C. (2000). The amygdala theory of autism. Neuroscience and biobehavioral reviews, 24, 355–364. doi:10.1016/S0149-7634(00)00011-7.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Bechara, A., Damasio, H., & Damasio, A. R. (2003). Role of the amygdala in decision-making. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 985, 356–369.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Bechara, A., Tranel, D., Damasio, H., & Damasio, A. R. (1996). Failure to respond autonomically to anticipated future outcomes following damage to prefrontal cortex. Cerebral Cortex (New York, NY), 6, 215–225. doi:10.1093/cercor/6.2.215.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Bernier, R., Dawson, G., Panagiotides, H., & Webb, S. (2005). Individuals with autism spectrum disorder show normal responses to a fear potential startle paradigm. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 35, 575–583. doi:10.1007/s10803-005-0002-0.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Beversdorf, D. Q., Anderson, J. M., Manning, S. E., Anderson, S. L., Dordgren, R. E., Felopulos, G. J., et al. (1998). The effect of semantic and emotional context on written recall for verbal language in high-functioning adults with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 65, 685–692. doi:10.1136/jnnp.65.5.685.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Corden, B., Chilvers, R., & Skuse, D. (2008). Emotional modulation of perception in Asperger’s syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 38, 1072–1080. doi:10.1007/s10803-007-0485-y.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Corona, R., Dissanayake, C., Arbelle, S., Wellington, P., & Sigman, M. (1998). Is affect aversive to young children with autism? Behavioural and cardiac responses to experimenter distress. Child Development, 69, 1494–1502.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  14. De Martino, B., Kalish, R., Rees, G., & Dolan, R. J. (2008). Enhanced processing of threat stimuli under limited attentional resources. Cerebral Cortex, 19, 127–133.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Deruelle, C., Hubert, B., Santos, A., & Wicker, B. (2008). Negative emotion does not enhance recall skills in adults with autistic spectrum disorders. Autism Research, 1, 91–97. doi:10.1002/aur.13.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Frith, U. (2003). Autism: Explaining the enigma (2nd ed.). Oxford: Blackwell Publishing.Google Scholar
  17. Gaigg, S. B., & Bowler, D. M. (2007). Differential fear conditioning in Asperger’s syndrome: Implications for an amygdala theory of autism. Neuropsychologia, 45, 2125–2134. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.01.012.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Gaigg, S. B., & Bowler, D. M. (2008). Free recall and forgetting of emotionally arousing words in autism spectrum disorder. Neuropsychologia, 46, 2336–2343. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.03.008.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Gaigg, S. B., & Bowler, D. M. (2009). Illusory memories of emotionally charged words in autism spectrum disorder: Further evidence for atypical emotion processing outside the social domain. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. doi:1007/s10803-009-0710-y.
  20. Hamann, S. (2001). Cognitive and neural mechanisms of emotional memory. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 5, 394–400. doi:10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01707-1.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Hobson, R. P. (2002). The cradle of thought. London: Pan Macmillan.Google Scholar
  22. Johnson, S. A., Yechiam, E., Murphy, R. R., Queller, S., & Stout, J. C. (2006). Motivational processes and autonomic responsivity in Asperger’s disorder: Evidence from the Iowa Gambling Task. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 12, 668–676.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  23. Keil, A., & Ihssen, N. (2004). Identification facilitation for emotionally arousing verbs during the attentional blink. Emotion (Washington, DC), 4, 23–35. doi:10.1037/1528-3542.4.1.23.Google Scholar
  24. Kučera, H., & Francis, W. (1967). Computational analysis of present-day American English. Providence, RI: Brown University Press.Google Scholar
  25. LeDoux, J. E. (1994). Emotion, memory and the brain. Scientific American, 270, 32–39.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Leslie, A. M., & Frith, U. (1990). Prospects for cognitive neuropsychology of autism: Hobson’s choice. Psychological Review, 97, 122–131. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.97.1.122.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Lord, C., Rutter, M., Goode, S., Heemsbergen, J., Jordan, H., Mawhood, L., et al. (1989). Autism diagnostic observation schedule: A standardized observation of communicative and social behaviour. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 19, 185–212. doi:10.1007/BF02211841.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Loveland, K. A. (2005). Social-emotional impairment and self-regulation in autism spectrum disorders. In J. Nadel & D. Muir (Eds.), Emotional development (pp. 365–382). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  29. McGaugh, J. L. (2000). Memory—a century of consolidation. Science, 287, 248–251. doi:10.1126/science.287.5451.248.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Ozonoff, S., Pennington, B. F., & Rogers, S. J. (1990). Are there emotion perception deficits in young autistic children? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 31, 343–361. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.1990.tb01574.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Phelps, E. A. (2004). Human emotion and memory: Interactions of the amygdala and hippocampal complex. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 14, 198–202. doi:10.1016/j.conb.2004.03.015.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Psychology Software Tools (1996–2002). E-Prime 1.1 (1.1.4.1).Google Scholar
  33. Raymond, J. E., Shapiro, K. L., & Arnell, K. M. (1992). Temporary suppression of visual processing in an RSVP Task: An attentional blink? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 18, 849–860. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.18.3.849.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Schultz, R. T. (2005). Developmental deficits in social perception in autism: The role of the amygdala and fusiform face area. International Journal of Developmental Neuroscience, 23, 125–141. doi:10.1016/j.ijdevneu.2004.12.012.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. South, M., Ozonoff, S., Suchy, Y., Kesner, R. P., McMahon, W. M., & Lainhart, J. E. (2008). Intact emotion facilitation for non-social stimuli in autism: Is amygdala impairment in autism specific for social information? Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 14, 42–54. doi:10.1017/S1355617708080107.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. The Psychological Corporation. (2000). Wechsler adult intelligence scale III UK edition (WAIS III UK ) (3rd ed.). London: The Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2009

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Autism Research Group, Department of PsychologyCity UniversityLondonUK

Personalised recommendations