Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders

, Volume 39, Issue 7, pp 1031–1038 | Cite as

Illusory Memories of Emotionally Charged Words in Autism Spectrum Disorder: Further Evidence for Atypical Emotion Processing Outside the Social Domain

original paper

Abstract

Recent evidence suggests that individuals with ASD may not accumulate distinct representations of emotional information throughout development. On the basis of this observation we predicted that such individuals would not be any less likely to falsely remember emotionally significant as compared to neutral words when such illusory memories are induced by asking participants to study lists of words that are orthographically associated to these words. Our findings showed that typical participants are far less likely to experience illusory memories of emotionally charged as compared to neutral words. Individuals with ASD, on the other hand, did not exhibit this emotional modulation of false memories. We discuss this finding in relation to the role of emotional processing atypicalities in ASD.

Keywords

Autism Spectrum Disorder Memory Orthographic associations Emotional processing Illusory memories 

References

  1. American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (DSM-IV-TR). Washington (DC), USA: Author.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Baron-Cohen, S. (1995). Mindblindness: An essay on autism and theory of mind. Cambridge: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  3. Baron-Cohen, S., Ring, H. A., Bullmore, E. T., Wheelwright, S., Ashwin, C., & Williams, S. C. (2000). The amygdala theory of autism. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 24, 355–364. doi: 10.1016/S0149-7634(00)00011-7.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Ben Shalom, D., Mostofsky, S. H., Hazlett, R. L., Goldberg, M. C., McLeod, D. R., & Hoehn-Saric, R. (2003). Intact galvanic skin responses and impaired self-reports in response to emotional pictures in high-functioning autism. New York Academy of Sciences, 985, 501–504.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Bernier, R., Dawson, G., Panagiotides, H., & Webb, S. (2005). Individuals with Autism Spectrum Disorder show normal responses to a fear potential startle paradigm. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 35, 575–583. doi: 10.1007/s10803-005-0002-0.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Beversdorf, D. Q., Anderson, J. M., Manning, S. E., Anderson, S. L., Dordgren, R. E., Felopulos, G. J., et al. (1998). The effect of semantic and emotional context on written recall for verbal language in high-functioning adults with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 65, 685–692. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.65.5.685.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Beversdorf, D. Q., Smith, B. W., Crucian, G. P., Anderson, J. M., Keillor, J. M., Barrett, A. M., et al. (2000). Increased discrimination of “false memories” in autism spectrum disorder. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 97, 8734–8737. doi: 10.1073/pnas.97.15.8734.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Bowler, D. M., Gardiner, J. M., Grice, S., & Saavalainen, P. (2000). Memory illusions: False recall and recognition in adults with Asperger’s syndrome. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 109, 663–672. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.109.4.663.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Bradley, M. M., Greenwald, M. K., Petry, M. C., & Lang, P. J. (1992). Remembering pictures: Pleasure and arousal in memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 18, 379–390. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.18.2.379.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Cahill, L., & McGaugh, L. (1998). Mechanisms of emotional arousal and lasting declarative memory. Trends in Neurosciences, 21, 294–299. doi: 10.1016/S0166-2236(97)01214-9.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Corona, R., Dissanayake, C., Arbelle, S., Wellington, P., & Sigman, M. (1998). Is affect aversive to young children with autism? Behavioural and cardiac responses to experimenter distress. Child Development, 69, 1494–1502.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  12. Deese, J. (1959). On the prediction of occurrence of particular verbal intrusions in immediate recall. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 58, 17–22. doi: 10.1037/h0046671.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Deruelle, C., Hubert, B., Santos, A., & Wicker, B. (2008). Negative emotion does not enhance recall skills in adults with autistic spectrum disorders. Autism Research, 1, 91–96.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Frith, U. (2003). Autism: Explaining the enigma (2nd ed.). Oxford: Blackwell Publishing.Google Scholar
  15. Gaigg, S. B., & Bowler, D. M. (2007). Differential fear conditioning in Asperger’s syndrome: Implications for an amygdala theory of autism. Neuropsychologia, 45, 2125–2134.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Gaigg, S. B., & Bowler, D. M. (2008). Free recall and forgetting of emotionally arousing words in autism spectrum disorder. Neuropsychologia, 46, 2336–2343. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.03.008.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Heuer, F., & Reisberg, D. (1990). Vivid memories of emotional events: The accuracy of remembered minutiae. Memory & Cognition, 18, 496–506.Google Scholar
  18. Hillier, A., Campbell, H., Keillor, J., Phillips, N., & Beversdorf, D. Q. (2007). Decreased false memory for visually presented shapes and symbols among adults on the autism spectrum. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 29, 610–616. doi: 10.1080/13803390600878760.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Hillier, A., Carpenter, A., Smith, R.M., Berntson, G.G., & Beversdorf, D.Q.(2006, June) Electrodermal responsiveness to affective stimuli among those on the autism spectrum. Poster session presented at the International Meeting for Autism Research, Montreal, Canada.Google Scholar
  20. Hobson, R. P. (2002). The cradle of thought. London: Pan Macmillan.Google Scholar
  21. Hobson, R. P., Ouston, J., & Lee, A. (1988). What’s in a face? The case of autism. The British Journal of Psychology, 79, 441–453.Google Scholar
  22. Johnson, S. A., Yechiam, E., Murphy, R. R., Queller, S., & Stout, J. C. (2006). Motivational processes and autonomic responsivity in Asperger’s disorder: Evidence from the Iowa Gambling Task. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 12, 668–676.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  23. Joseph, R. M., & Tager-Flusberg, H. (1997). An investigation of attention and affect in children with autism and down syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 27, 385–396. doi: 10.1023/A:1025853321118.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Kasari, C., Sigman, M., Mundy, P., & Yirmiya, N. (1990). Affective sharing in the context of joint attention interactions of normal, autistic, and mentally retarded children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 20, 87–100. doi: 10.1007/BF02206859.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Kensinger, E. A., & Corkin, S. (2003). Memory enhancement for emotional words: Are emotional words more vividly remembered than neutral words? Memory & Cognition, 31, 1169–1180.Google Scholar
  26. Kučera, H., & Francis, W. (1967). Computational analysis of present-day American English. Providence, RI: Brown University Press.Google Scholar
  27. LeDoux, J. (1996). The emotional brain. New York: Simon and Schuster.Google Scholar
  28. Leslie, A. M., & Frith, U. (1990). Prospects for cognitive neuropsychology of autism: Hobson’s Choice. Psychological Review, 97, 122–131. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.97.1.122.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Lord, C., Rutter, M., & Couteur, A. L. (1994). Autism diagnostic interview-revised: A revised version of a diagnostic interview for caregivers of individuals with possible pervasive developmental disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 24, 659–685. doi: 10.1007/BF02172145.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Lord, C., Rutter, M., Goode, S., Heemsbergen, J., Jordan, H., Mawhood, L., et al. (1989). Autism diagnostic observation schedule: A standardized observation of communicative and social behaviour. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 19, 185–212. doi: 10.1007/BF02211841.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Loveland, K. A. (2005). Social-emotional impairment and self-regulation in autism spectrum disorders. In J. Nadel & D. Muir (Eds.), Emotional Development (pp. 365–382). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  32. Magyar, C. I., & Pandolfi, V. (2007). Factor structure evaluation of the childhood autism rating scale. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37, 1787–1794. doi: 10.1007/s10803-006-0313-9.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Moore, D. G., Hobson, R. P., & Lee, A. (1997). Components of person perception: An investigation with autistic, non-autistic retarded and typically developing children and adolescents. The British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 15, 401–423.Google Scholar
  34. Mottron, L., & Burack, J. (2001). Enhanced perceptual functioning in the development of autism. In J. Burack, A. Charman, N. Yirmiya, & P. R. Zelazo (Eds.), Development and Autism: Perspectives from theory and research. New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
  35. Mottron, L., Dawson, M., Soulières, I., Hubert, B., & Burack, J. (2006). Enhanced perceptual functioning in autism: an update, and eight principles of autistic perception. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36, 27–43. doi: 10.1007/s10803-005-0040-7.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Mundy, P., & Sigman, M. (1989). Specifying the nature of the social impairment in autism. In G. Dawson (Ed.), Autism: Nature diagnosis and treatment (pp. 3–21). New York: The Guildford Press.Google Scholar
  37. Pesta, B. J., Murphy, M. D., & Sanders, R. E. (2001). Are emotionally charged lures immune to false memory? Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 27, 328–338. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.27.2.328.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Reisberg, D., & Hertel, P. (Eds.). (2004). Memory and Emotion. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  39. Robins, D. L., Fein, D., Barton, M. L., & Green, J. A. (2001). The modified checklist for autism in toddlers: An initial study investigating the early detection of autism and pervasive developmental disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 31, 131–144. doi: 10.1023/A:1010738829569.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Roediger, H. L., III, & McDermott, K. B. (1995). Creating false memories: Remembering words not presented in lists. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 21, 803–814. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.21.4.803.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Schultz, R. T. (2005). Developmental deficits in social perception in autism: the role of the amygdala and fusiform face area. International Journal of Developmental Neuroscience, 23, 125–141. doi: 10.1016/j.ijdevneu.2004.12.012.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Shah, A., & Frith, U. (1993). Why do autistic individuals show superior performance on the block design task? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 34, 1351–1364. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1993.tb02095.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Sigman, M. D., Kasari, C., Kwon, J., & Yirmiya, N. (1992). Responses to the negative emotions of others by autistic, mentally retarded, and normal children. Child Development, 63, 796–807. doi: 10.2307/1131234.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. South, M., Ozonoff, S., Suchy, Y., Kesner, R. P., McMahon, W. M., & Lainhart, J. E. (2008). Intact emotion facilitation for non-social stimuli in autism: Is amygdala impairment in autism specific for social information? Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 14, 42–54. doi: 10.1017/S1355617708080107.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. The Psychological Corporation.(2000). Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale III UK edition (WAIS III UK ). 3rd edition. London: The Psychological Corporation; 2000.Google Scholar
  46. Uttl, B., Ohta, N., & Siegenthaler, A. L. (Eds.). (2006). Memory and emotion: Interdisciplinary perspectives. Oxford: Blackwell Publishing.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2009

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Autism Research Group, Department of PsychologyCity UniversityLondonUK

Personalised recommendations