Advertisement

Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders

, Volume 38, Issue 10, pp 1989–1997 | Cite as

Brief Report: Atypical Social Cognition and Social Behaviours in Autism Spectrum Disorder: A Different Way of Processing Rather than an Impairment

  • Kate O’ConnorEmail author
  • Ian Kirk
Brief Report

Abstract

A central question to autism research is whether autism is largely the result of an impairment in social cognition and/or motivation or the result of a more general processing difference. This review discusses problems with the “social deficit” model of autism spectrum disorder (ASD). It is suggested that superior attention to low-level perceptual information potentially coupled with decreased attention to global information may provide a more comprehensive explanation for atypical social behaviours in ASD. This processing style may reflect increased activation of occipital-temporal regions and reduced functional (and possibly anatomical) connectivity. It is concluded that atypical social behaviours in ASD are more likely to be a consequence reflective of a general processing difference than impairment in social cognition and/or motivation.

Keywords

Autism Asperger’s syndrome Low-level perceptual processing Central coherence Social cognition 

Notes

Acknowledgments

Kate O’Connor would like to thank all the individuals with autism and AS she has met for their openness and insight which motivated her to write this review. Also thank you to Ian Kirk for his financial support and Joss O’Connor for editing an earlier version of the manuscript.

References

  1. Adophs, R., Sears, L., & Piven, J. (2001). Abnormal processing of social information from faces in autism. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 13(2), 232–240.Google Scholar
  2. Adrien, J., Lenoir, P., Martineau, J., Perrot, A., Hameury, L., Larmande, C., et al. (1993). Blind ratings of early symptoms of autism based upon family home movies. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 32(3), 617–626.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  3. APA. (1994). DSM-IV diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.Google Scholar
  4. Aldridge, M., Stone, K., Sweeny, M., & Bower, T. (2000). Preverbal children with autism understand the intentions of others. Developmental Science, 3(3), 294–301.Google Scholar
  5. Asperger, H. (1944). Die Autistischen Psychopathen. Kindesalter, Archiv. fur Psychiatrie und Nervenkrankheiten, 117, 76–136.Google Scholar
  6. Attwood, T. (1998). Asperger’s syndrome: A guide for parents and professionals (1st ed.). London: Jessica-Kingsley Publishers Ltd.Google Scholar
  7. Bailey, A., Braeutigam, S., Jousmaki, V., & Swithenby, S. (2005). Abnormal activation of face processing systems at early and intermediate latency in individuals with autism spectrum disorder: A magnetoencephalographic study. European Journal of Neuroscience, 21(9), 2575–2585.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  8. Barnea-Goraly, N., Kwon, H., Menon, V., Eliez, S., Lotspeich, L., & Reiss, A. (2004). White matter structure in autism: Preliminary evidence from diffusion tensor imaging. Biological Psychiatry, 55, 323–326.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  9. Baron-Cohen, S., Cox, A., Baird, G., Swettenham, J., Nightengale, N., & Morgan, K. (1996). Psychological markers of autism at 18 months of age in a large population. British Journal of Psychiatry, 168, 158–163.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  10. Baron-Cohen, S., Jolliffe, T., Mortimore, C., & Robertson, M. (1997). Another advanced test of theory of mind: Evidence from very high functioning adults with autism or Asperger syndrome. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 38, 813–822.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  11. Baron-Cohen, S., Ring, H., Wheelwright, S., Bullmore, E., Brammer, M., Simmons, A., et al. (1999). Social intelligence in the normal and autistic brain: An fMRI study. European Journal of Neuroscience, 11, 1891–1898.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  12. Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Hill, J., Raste, Y., & Plumb, I. (2001). The “reading the mind in the eyes” test revised version: A study with normal adults and adults with Asperger’s syndrome or high-functioning autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 42(2), 241–251.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  13. Basser, P. (1995). Inferring microstructural features and the physiological state of tissues from diffusion-weighted images. NMR in Biomedicne, 8, 333–344.Google Scholar
  14. Beaulieu, C. (2002). The basis of anisotropic water diffusion in the nervous system–a technical review. NMR in Biomedicine, 15, 435–455.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  15. Beckett, C., Bredenkamp, D., Castle, J., Groothues, C., O’Connor, T., Rutter, M., et al. (2002). Behaviour patterns associated with institutional deprivation: A study of children adopted from Romania. Journal of Developmental and Behavioural Pediatrics, 23(5), 297–303.Google Scholar
  16. Belmonte, M. K., Cook, E. H., Anderson, G. M., Rubenstein, J. L. R., Greenough, W. T., Beckel-Mitchener, A., et al. (2004). Autism as a disorder of neural information processing: Directions for research and targets for therapy. Molecular Psychiatry, 9, 646–663.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  17. Belmonte, M., & Yurgelun-Todd, D. (2003). Functional anatomy of impaired selective attention and compensatory processing in autism. Cognitive Brain Research, 17(3), 651–664.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  18. Birch, J. (2003). Congratulations! it’s Asperger’s syndrome. London: Jessica-KIngsley Publishers.Google Scholar
  19. Boddaert, N., Chabane, N., Gervais, H., Good, C., Bourgeois, M., Plumet, M.-H., et al. (2004). Superior temporal sulcus anatomical abnormalities in childhood autism: A voxel-based morphometry MRI study. NeuroImage, 23, 364–369.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  20. Bodfish, J., Symons, F., Parker, D., & Lewis, M. (2000). Varieties of repetitive behaviour in autism: Comparisons to mental retardation. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 30, 237–243.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  21. Bowler, D. (1992). Theory of mind in Asperger syndrome. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 33, 877–895.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  22. Braverman, M., Fein, D., Lucci, D., & Waterhouse, L. (1989). Affect comprehension in children with pervasive developmental disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 19(2), 301–316.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  23. Carper, R., Moses, P., Tigue, Z., & Courchesne, E. (2002). Cerebral lobes in autism: Early hyperplasia and abnormal age effects. NeuroImage, 16(4), 1038–1051.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  24. Castelli, F. (2005). Understanding emotions from standardized facial expressions in autism and normal development. Autism, 9(4), 428–449.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  25. Castelli, F., Frith, C., Happe, F., & Frith, U. (2002). Autism, Asperger’s syndrome and brain mechanisms for the attribution of mental states to animated shapes. Brain, 125, 1839–1849.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  26. Cesaroni, L., & Garber, M. (1991). Exploring the experience of autism through firsthand accounts. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 21, 303–313.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  27. Chawarska, K, & Volkmar, F. (2007). Impairments in monkey and human face recognition in 2-year-old toddlers with autism spectrum disorder and developmental delay. Developmental Science, 10(2), 266–279.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  28. Cherkassky, V., Kana, R., Keller, T., & Just, M. (2006). Functional connectivity in a baseline resting-state network in autism. NeuroReport.Google Scholar
  29. Chung, M., Dalton, K., Alexander, A., & Davidson, R. (2004). Less white matter concentration in autism: 2D voxel-based morphometry. NeuroImage, 23, 242–251.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  30. Courchesne, E. (2004). Brain development in autism: Early overgrowth followed by premature arrest of growth. Mental Retardation and Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews, 10(2), 106–111.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  31. Courchesne, E., Carper, R., & Akshoomoff, N. (2003). Evidence of brain overgrowth in the first year of life in autism. JAMA, 290, 337–344.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  32. Courchesne, E., Karns, C., & Davis, H. (2001). Unusual brain growth patterns in early life in patients with autistic disorder: An MRI study. Neurology, 57, 245–254.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  33. Courchesne, E., & Pierce, K. (2005). Why the frontal cortex in autism might be talking only to itself: Local over-connectivity but long-distance disconnection. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 15(2), 225–230.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  34. Critchley, H., Daly, E., Bullmore, E., Williams, S., van Amelsvoort, T., Robertson, D., et al. (2000). The functional neuroanatomy of social behaviour: Changes in cerebral blood flow when people with autistic disorder process facial expressions. Brain, 123, 2203–2212.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  35. Dahlgren, S., & Trillingsgaard, A. (1996). Theory of mind in non-retarded children with autism and AS. A research note. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 37, 759–763.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  36. Dalton, K., Nacewicz, B., Johnstone, T., Schaefer, H., Gernsbacher, M., Goldsmith, H., et al. (2005). Gaze fixation and the neural circuitry of face processing in autism. Nature Neuroscience, 24(4), 1–11.Google Scholar
  37. Dapretto, M., Davies, M., Pfeifer, J., Scott, A., Sigman, M., Bookheimer, S., et al. (2005). Understanding emotions in others: Mirror neuron dysfunction in children with autism spectrum disorders. Nature Neuroscience, 30, 1–3.Google Scholar
  38. Davies, S., Bishop, D., Manstead, A., & Digby, T. (1994). Face perception in children with autism and Asperger’s syndrome. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 35(6), 1033–1057.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  39. Dawson, G., Webb, S., & McPartland, J. (2005). Understanding the nature of face processing impairment in autism: Insights from behavioural and electrophysiological studies. Developmental Neuropsychology, 27(3), 403–424.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  40. Dementieva, Y., Vance, D., Donnelly, S., & Elston, L. (2005). Accelerated head growth in early development of individuals with autism. Pediatric Neurology, 32(2), 102–108.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  41. Frith, C. (2003). What do imaging studies tell us about the neural basis of autism? In Bock, & Goode (Eds.), Autism: Neural basis and treatment possibilities (pp. 149–176). Chichester, UK: John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
  42. Frith, U. (1989). Autism: Explaining the enigma. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
  43. Grandin, T. (1995). Thinking in pictures and other reports from my life with autism. New York: Vintage Books Inc.Google Scholar
  44. Grossman, J., Klin, A., Carter, A., & Volkmar, F. (2000). Verbal bias in recognition of facial emotions in children with Asperger syndrome. The Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 41, 368–379.Google Scholar
  45. Hadjikhani, N., Joseph, R., Snyder, J., Chabris, C., Clark, J., Steele, S., et al. (2004). Activation of the fusiform gyrus when individuals with autism spectrum disorder view faces. NeuroImage, 22, 1141–1150.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  46. Hall, G., Szechtman, H., & Nahmias, C. (2003). Enhanced salience and emotion recognition in autism: A PET study. American Journal of Psychiatry, 160(8), 1439–1441.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  47. Happe, F. (1994). An advanced test of theory of mind: Understanding of story characters’ thoughts and feelings by able autistic, mentally handicapped and normal children and adults. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 24, 129–154.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  48. Happe, F., Ehlers, S., Fletcher, S., Frith, U., Johansson, M., & Gillberg, C. (1996). ‘Theory of mind’ in the brain. Evidence from a PET scan study of Asperger syndrome. Neuroreport, 8, 197–201.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Happe, F., & Frith, U. (2006). The weak coherence account: Detail-focused cognitive style in autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 28(1), 1–21.Google Scholar
  50. Hazlett, E., Buchsbaum, M., Hsieh, P., Haznedar, M., Platoli, J., LiCalzi, E., et al. (2004). Regional glucose metabolism within cortical Brodmann areas in healthy individuals and autistic patients. Neuopsychobiology, 49, 115–125.Google Scholar
  51. Hobson, R. (1986). The autistic child’s appraisal of expressions of emotion. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 27(3), 321–342.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  52. Hobson, R., Ouston, J., & Lee, A. (1988). What’s in a face? The case of autism. British Journal of Psychology, 79, 441–453.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  53. Hubl, D., Bolte, S., Feineis-Matthews, S., Lanfermann, H., Federspiel, A., Strik, W., et al. (2003). Functional imbalance of visual pathways indicates alternative face processing strategies in autism. Neurology, 61, 1232–1237.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  54. Jemel, B., Mottron, L., & Dawson, M. (2006). Impaired face processing in autism: Fact or artifact? Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36(1), 91–106.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  55. Jolliffe, T., & Baron-Cohen, S. (1997). Are people with autism and Asperger’s syndrome faster than normal on the Embedded Figures test? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 38, 527–534.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  56. Jones, R., Zahl, A., & Huws, J. (2001). First-hand accounts of emotional experiences in autism: A qualitative analysis. Disability and Society, 16(3), 393–401.Google Scholar
  57. Just, M., Cherkassky, V., Keller, T., Kana, R., & Minshew, N. (2006). Functional and anatomical cortical underconnectivity in autism: Evidence form an fMRI study of an executive function task and corpus callosum morphometry. Cerebral Cortex.Google Scholar
  58. Just, M., Cherkassky, V., Keller, T., & Minshew, N. (2004). Cortical activation and synchronization during sentence comprehension in high-functioning autism: Evidence of underconnectivity. Brain, 127, 1811–1821.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  59. Kana, R., Keller, T., Cherkassky, V., Minshew, N., & Just, M. (2006b). Sentence comprehension in autism: Thinking in pictures with decreased functional connectivity. Brain (in press).Google Scholar
  60. Kanner, L. (1943). Autistic disturbances of affective contact. Nervous Child, 2, 217–250.Google Scholar
  61. Keller, T., Kana, R., & Just, M. (2006). A developmental study of the structural integrity of white matter in autism. NeuroReport.Google Scholar
  62. Klin, A., Jones, W., Schultz, R., & Volkmar, F. (2003). The enactive mind—from actions to cognition: Lessons from autism. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London Series B, 358, 345–360.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  63. Klin, A., Jones, W., Schultz, R., Volkmar, F., & Cohen, D. (2002). Visual fixation patterns during viewing of naturalistic social situations as predictors of social competence in individuals with autism. Archives of General Psychiatry, 59(9), 809–816.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  64. Koshino, H., Carpenter, P., Minshew, N., Cherkassky, V., Keller, T., & Just, M. (2005). Functional connectivity in an fMRI working memory task in high-functioning autism. NeuroImage, 24, 810–821.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  65. Lahaie, A., Mottron, L., Arguin, M., Berthiaume, C., Jemel, B., & Saumier, D. (2006). Face perception in high-functioning autistic adults: Evidence for superior processing of face parts, not for a configural face processing deficit. Neuropsychology, 20(1), 30–41.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  66. Leekman, S., & Prior, M. (1994). Can autistic children distinguish lies from jokes? A second look at second-order belief attribution. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 35, 901–915.Google Scholar
  67. Loveland, K., Tunali-Kotoski, B., Chen, R., Brelsford, K., & Ortegon, J. (1995). Intermodal perception of affect by persons with autism or down syndrome. Development and Psychopathology, 7, 409–418.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  68. Luna, B., Minshew, N., Garver, K., Lazar, N., Thulborn, K., Eddy, W., et al. (2002). Neocortical system abnormalities in autism: An fMRI study of spatial working memory. Neurology, 59, 834–840.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  69. Maestro, S., Muratin, F., Cavallaro, M., Pei, F., Stern, D., Golse, B., et al. (2002). Attentional skills during the first 6 months of age in autism spectrum disorder. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 4(10), 1239–1245.Google Scholar
  70. Martin, I., & McDonald, S. (2004). An exploration of causes of non-literal language problems in individuals with Asperger syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 34, 311–328.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  71. McAlonan, M., Cheung, V., Cheung, C., Suckling, J., Grace, L., Tai, K., et al. (2005). Mapping the brain in autism. A voxel-based MrI study of volumetric differences and intercorrelations in autism. Journal of Neurology, 128(2), 268–276.Google Scholar
  72. McPartland, J., Dawson, G., Webb, S., Panagiotides, H., & Carver, L. (2004). Event-related brain potentials reveal anomalies in temporal processing of faces in autism spectrum disorder. The Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45(7), 1235–1245.Google Scholar
  73. Miller, J. (2003). Women from another planet? Our lives in the Universe of autism. Milan: Dancing Mind Books.Google Scholar
  74. Minshew, N., Goldstein, G., & Siegal, D. (1995). Speech and language in high-functioning autistic individuals. Neuropsychology, 9, 255–261.Google Scholar
  75. Morgan, B., Mayberry, M., & Durkin, K. (2003). Weak central coherence, poor joint attention and low verbal ability: Independent deficits in early autism. Developmental Psychology, 39, 646–656.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  76. Mottron, L., Peretz, I., & Menard, E. (2000). Local and global processing of music in high-functioning persons with autism: Beyond central coherence? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 41(8), 1057–1065.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  77. Mottron, L., Dawson, M., Soulieres, I., Hubert, B., & Burack, J. (2006). Enhanced perceptual functioning in autism: An update and eight principles of autistic perception. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36(1), 27–43.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  78. Müller, R., Kleinhans, N., Kemmotsu, N., Pierce, K., & Courchesne, E. (2003). Abnormal variability and distribution of functional maps in autism: An fMRI study of visuomotor learning. American Journal of Psychiatry, 160, 1847–1862.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  79. Mundy, P., & Gomes, A. (1998). Individuals differences in joint attention skill devlopment in the second year. Infant Behaviour and Development, 21(3), 469–482.Google Scholar
  80. Mundy, P., Sigman, M., & Kasari, C. (1990). A longitudinal study of joint attention and language development in autistic children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 20(1), 115–128.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  81. Nishitani, N., Avaikainen, S., & Hari, R. (2004). Abnormal imitation-related cortical activation sequences in Asperger’s syndrome. Annuals of Neurology, 55, 558–562.Google Scholar
  82. O’Connor, K. (2007). Brief report: Impaired identification of discrepancies between expressive faces and voices in Adults with Asperger’s syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37(10), 2008–2013.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  83. O’Connor, K., Hamm, J., & Kirk, I. (2005). The neurophysiological correlates of face processing in adults and children with Asperger’s syndrome. Brain and Cognition, 59(1), 82–95.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  84. O’Connor, T., Rutter, M., Beckett, C., Keaveney, L., & Kreppner, J. (2000). The effects of global and severe privation on cognitive competence: Extension and longitudinal follow-up. Child Development, 71, 376–390.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  85. Ogai, M., Matsumoto, H., Suzuki, K., Ozawa, F., Fukuda, R., Uchiyama, I., et al. (2003). fMRI study of recognition of facial expressions in high-functioning autistic patients. NeuroReport, 14(4), 559–563.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  86. Osterling, J., & Dawson, G. (1994). Early recognition of children with autism: A study of first birthday home videotapes. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 24, 247–257.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  87. Ozonoff, S., & Miller, G. (1996). An exploration of right-hemisphere contributions to the pragmatic impairments in autism. Brain and Language, 52, 411–434.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  88. Ozonoff, S., Pennington, B., & Rogers, S. (1990). Executive function deficits in high-functioning autistic individuals: Relationship to theory of mind. The Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 32(7), 1081–1105.Google Scholar
  89. Pelphrey, K., Sasson, N., Reznick, S., Paul, G., Goldman, B., & Piven, J. (2002). Visual scanning of faces in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 32(4), 249–261.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  90. Pierce, K., Haist, F., Sedaghat, F., & Courchesne, E. (2004). The brain response to personally familiar faces in autism: Findings of fusiform activity and beyond. Brain, 127(12), 2703–2716.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  91. Pierce, K., Muller, R.-A., Ambrose, J., Allen, G., & Courchesne, E. (2001). Face processing occurs outside the fusiform ‘face area’ in autism: Evidence from functional MRI. Brain, 124(10), 2059–2073.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  92. Plaisted, K., Swettenham, J., & Rees, L. (1999). Children with autism show local precedence in a divided attention task and global precedence in a selective attention task. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 40(5), 733–742.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  93. Rinehart, N., Bradshaw, J., Moss, S., Brereton, A., & Tonge, B. (2001). A deficit in shifting attention present in high-functioning autism but not Asperger’s disorder. Autism, 5(1), 67–80.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  94. Ring, H., Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Williams, S., Brammer, M., Andrew, C., et al. (1999). Cerebral correlates of preserved cognitive skills in autism: A functional MRI study of embedded figures task performance. Brain, 122, 1305–1315.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  95. Rogers, S., Hepburn, S., & Wehner, E. (2003). Parent reports of sensory symptoms in toddlers with autism and those with other developmental disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 33(6), 631–642.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  96. Ropar, D., & Mitchell, P. (2001). Susceptibility to illusions and performance on visospatial tasks in individuals with autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 42, 539–549.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  97. Sainsbury, C. (2000). Martian in the playground. London: The Book Factory.Google Scholar
  98. Schultz, R. (2005). Developmental deficits in social perception in autism: The role of the amygdala and fusiform face area. International Journal of Developmental Neuroscience, 23, 125–141.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  99. Schultz, R. T., Gauthier, I., Klin, A., Fulbright, R. K., Anderson, A. W., Volkmar, F., et al. (2000). Abnormal ventral temporal cortical activity during face discrimination among individuals with autism and Asperger syndrome. Archives of General Psychiatry, 57, 331–340.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  100. Shah, A., & Frith, U. (1983). An islet of ability in autistic children: A research note. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 24, 613–620.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  101. Shah, A., & Frith, U. (1993). Why do autistic individuals show superior performance on the block design task? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 34(8), 1351–1364.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  102. Siller, M., & Sigman, M. (2002). The behaviours of parents of children with autism predict the subsequent development of their children’s communication. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 32(2), 77–89.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  103. Singer-Harris, N., Bellugi, U., Bates, E., Jones, W., & Rossen, M. (1997). Contrasting profiles of language development in children with Williams and Downs syndrome. Developmental Neuropsychology, 13(3), 345–370.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  104. Spezio, M., Adolphs, R., Hurley, R., & Piven, J. (2007). Analysis of face gaze in autism using “Bubbles”. Neuropsychologia, 45, 144–151.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  105. Tager-Flusberg, H., & Cooper, J. (1999). Present and future possibilities for defining a phenotype for specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 42, 1275–1278.Google Scholar
  106. Talay-Ongan, A., & Wood, K. (2000). Unusual sensory sensitivities in autism: A possible crosswords. International Journal of Disability, Development and Education, 47, 201–212.Google Scholar
  107. Vermeulen, P. (2001). Autistic thinking—this is the title (1st ed.). London: Jessica-Kingsley Publishers.Google Scholar
  108. Villalobos, M., Mizuno, A., Dahl, B., Kemmotsu, N., & Muller, R.-A. (2005). Reduced functional connectivity between VI and inferior frontal cortex associated with visuomotor performance in autism. NeuroImage, 25, 916–925.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  109. Waiter, G., Williams, J., Murray, A., Gilchrist, A., Perrett, D., & Whiten, A. (2005). Structural white matter deficits in high-functioning individuals with autistic spectrum disorder: A voxel-based investigation. NeuroImage, 24, 455–461.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  110. Wang, A., Dapretto, M., Hariri, A., Sigman, M., & Bookheimer, S. (2004). Neural correlates of facial affect processing in children and adolescents with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 43(4), 481–490.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  111. Wang, A., Lee, S., Sigman, M., & Dapretto, M. (2006). Neural basis of irony comprehension in children with autism: The role of prosody and context. Brain, 129, 932–943.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  112. Watling, R., Deitz, J., & White, O. (2001). Comparison of sensory profile scores of young children with and without autism spectrum diosrders. American Journal of Occupational Therapy, 55, 416–423.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  113. Weeks, S., & Hobson, R. (1987). The salience of facial expression for autistic children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 28(1), 137–152.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  114. WHO. (1992). The ICD-10 classification of mental and behavioural disorders. Clinical descriptions and diagnostic guidelines. Geneva.Google Scholar
  115. Williams, D. (1995). Somebody somewhere. New York: Times Books.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2008

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of PsychologyThe University of AucklandAucklandNew Zealand

Personalised recommendations