Advertisement

Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders

, Volume 38, Issue 7, pp 1371–1381 | Cite as

Scan Path Differences and Similarities During Emotion Perception in those With and Without Autism Spectrum Disorders

Original Paper

Abstract

Typical adults use predictable scan patterns while observing faces. Some research suggests that people with autism spectrum disorders (ASD) instead attend to eyes less, and perhaps to the mouth more. The current experiment was designed as a direct measure of scan paths that people with and without ASD use when identifying simple and complex emotions. Participants saw photos of emotions and chose emotion labels. Scan paths were measured via infrared corneal reflectance. Both groups looked significantly longer at eyes than mouth, and neither overall looking time at eyes nor first fixations distinguished the groups. These results are contrary to suggestions that those with ASD attend preferentially to the mouth and avoid the eyes. Furthermore, there was no interaction between group and area of the face: the ratio of attention between eyes and mouth did not differ between the ASD and control groups. However, those with ASD looked at the eyes less than the control group when viewing complex emotions.

Keywords

Emotion perception Face perception Scan path Eye tracking 

References

  1. American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed.). Washington, D.C.: American Psychiatric Publishing, Inc.Google Scholar
  2. Baron-Cohen, S., Campbell, R., Karmiloff-Smith, A., Grant, J., & Walker, J. (1995). Are children with autism blind to the mentalistic significance of the eyes? British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 13, 379–398.Google Scholar
  3. Baron-Cohen, S., & Cross, P. (1992). Reading the eyes: Evidence for the role of perception in the development of a theory of mind. Mind and Language, 6, 173–186.Google Scholar
  4. Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., & Jolliffe, T. (1997). Is there a “language of the eyes”? Evidence from normal adults, and adults with autism or Asperger Syndrome. Visual Cognition, 4, 311–331.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Barton, J. J. S., Keenan, J. P., & Bass, T. (2001). Discrimination of spatial relations and features in faces: Effects of inversion and viewing duration. British Journal of Psychology, 92, 527–549.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Boucher, J. D., & Ekman, P. (1975). Facial areas and emotional information. Journal of Communication, 25, 21–29.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Boucher, J., & Lewis, V. (1992). Unfamiliar face recognition in relatively able autistic children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 33, 843–859.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Braverman, M., Fein, D., Lucci, D., & Waterhouse, L. (1989). Affect comprehension in children with pervasive developmental disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 17, 301–316.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Buitelaar, J. K., Van der Wees, M., Swabb-Barneveld, H., & Van der Gaag, R. J. (1999). Theory of mind and emotion-recognition functioning in autistic spectrum disorders and in psychiatric control and normal children. Development and Psychopathology, 11, 39–58.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Caldara, R., Schyns, P., Mayer, E., Smith, M. L., Gosselin, F., & Rossion, B. (2005). Does prosopagnosia take the eyes out of face representations? Evidence for a defect in representing diagnostic facial information following brain damage. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 17, 1652–1666.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Celani, G., Battacchi, M. W., & Arcidiacono, L. (1999). The understanding of the emotional meaning of facial expressions in people with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 29, 57–66.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Davies, G., Ellis, H., & Shepherd, J. (1977). Cue saliency in faces as assessed by the “photofit” technique. Perception, 6, 263–269.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. de Gelder, B., Vroomen, J., & van der Heide, L. (1991). Face recognition and lip-reading in autism. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 3, 69–86.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Dunlap, K. (1927). The role of eye muscles and mouth muscles in the expression of the emotions. Child Behaviour, Differential and Genetic Psychology, 2, 199–233.Google Scholar
  15. Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. V. (1971). Constants across cultures in the face and emotion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 17, 129.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A.-G., & Buchner, A. (2007). G*Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 39, 175–191.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  17. Fraser, I. H., Craig, G. L., & Parker, D. M. (1990). Reaction time measures of feature saliency in schematic faces. Perception, 19, 661–673.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Gepner, B., deGelder, B., & deSchonen, S. (1996). Face processing in autistics: Evidence for a generalised deficit? Child Neuropsychology, 2, 123–139.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Hadwin, J., Baron-Cohen, S., Howlin, P., & Hill, K. (1996). Can we teach children with autism to understand emotions, belief or pretence? Development and Psychopathology, 8, 345–365.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Haig, N. D. (1985). How faces differ – A new comparative technique. Perception, 14, 601–615.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Haig, N. D. (1986). Exploring recognition with interchanged facial features. Perception, 15, 235–247.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Hanawalt, N. G. (1944). The role of the upper and the lower parts of the face as the basis for judging facial expressions: II. In posed expressions and “candid camera” pictures. The Journal of General Psychology, 31, 23–36.Google Scholar
  23. Happé, F., & Frith, U. (2006). The weak central coherence account: Detail-focussed cognitive style in autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36, 5–25.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Hobson, R. P. (1986). The autistic childs appraisal of expressions of emotion. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 27, 321–342.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Hobson, R. P., Ouston, J., & Lee, A. (1988). What’s in a face? The case of autism. British Journal of Psychology, 79, 441–453.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  26. Howlin, P., Baron-Cohen, S., & Hadwin, J. (1999). Teaching children with autism to mind-read: A practical guide. Chichester: Wiley.Google Scholar
  27. Joseph, R. M., & Tanaka, J. (2003). Holistic and part-based face recognition in children with autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 44, 529–542.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Klin, A., Jones, W., Schultz, R., Volkmar, F., & Cohen, D. (2002). Visual fixation patterns during viewing of naturalistic social situations as predictors of social competence in individuals with autism. Archives of General Psychiatry, 59, 809–816.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Klin, A., Sparrow, S., de Bildt, A., Cicchetti, D., Cohen, D., & Volkmar, F. (1999). A normed study of face recognition in autism and related disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 29, 499–508.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Langdell, T. (1978). Recognition of faces – Approach to study of autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 19, 255–268.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Lord, C., Risi, S., Lambrecht, L., Cook, E. H., Leventhal, B. L., DiLavore, P. C., et al. (2000). The Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule–Generic: A standard measure of social and communication deficits associated with the spectrum of autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 30, 205–223.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Loveland, K. A., Tunali-Kotoski, B., Chen, Y. R., Ortegon, J., Pearson, D. A., Brelsford, K. A., et al. (1997). Emotion recognition in autism: Verbal and nonverbal information. Development and Psychopathology, 9, 579–593.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Nishimura, M., Rutherford, M. D., & Maurer, D. (2007). Evidence of configural face processing in adults with autism. Visual Cognition.Google Scholar
  34. Ozonoff, S., Pennington, B. F., & Rogers, S. J. (1990). Are there emotion perception deficits in young autistic children? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 31, 343–361.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Pelphrey, K. A., Sasson, N. J., Reznick, J. S., Paul, G., Goldman, B. D., & Piven, J. (2002). Visual scanning of faces in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 32, 249–261.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Plaisted, K., Saksida, L., Alcantara, J., & Weisblatt, E. (2003). Towards an understanding of the mechanisms of weak central coherence effects: Experiments in visual configural learning and auditory perception. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London Series B-Biological Sciences, 358, 375–386.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Rutherford, M. D., Clements, K., & Sekuler, A. B. (2007). Differences in discrimination of eye and mouth displacement in autism spectrum disorders. Vision Research, 47, 2099–2110.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Rutherford, M. D., & McIntosh, D. I. (2007). Rules versus prototype matching: Strategies of perception of emotional facial expressions in the autism spectrum. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37, 187–196.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Schyns, P. G., Bonnar, L., & Gosselin, F. (2002). Show me the features! Understanding recognition from the use of visual information. Psychological Science, 13, 402–409.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Sergent, J. (1984). Configural processing of faces in the left and right cerebral hemispheres. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 10, 554–572.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Silver, M., & Oakes, P. (2001). Evaluation of a new computer intervention to teach people with autism or Asperger syndrome to recognize and predict emotions in others. Autism, 5, 299–316.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Spezio, M. L., Adolphs, R., Hurley, R. S. E., & Piven, J. (2007a). Abnormal use of facial information in high-functioning autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37, 929–939.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Spezio, M. L., Adolphs, R., Hurley, R. S. E., & Piven, J. (2007b). Analysis of face gaze in autism with “Bubbles”. Neuropsychologia, 45, 144–151.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Sullivan, L. A., & Kirkpatrick, S. W. (1996). Facial interpretation and component consistency. Genetic, Social, and General Psychology Monographs, 122, 389–404.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  45. Tanaka, J. W., & Farah, M. J. (1993). Parts and wholes in face recognition. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: A Human Experimental Psychology, 46a, 225–245.Google Scholar
  46. Tantam, D., Monaghan, L., Nicholson, H., & Stirling, J. (1989). Autistic children’s ability to interpret faces: A research note. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 30, 623–630.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Teunisse, J. P., & de Gelder, B. (2001). Impaired categorical perception of facial expressions in high-functioning adolescents with autism. Child Neuropsychology, 7, 1–14.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  48. Teunisse, J. P., & de Gelder, B. (2003). Face processing in adolescents with autistic disorder: The inversion and composite effects. Brain and Cognition, 52, 285–294.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. van der Geest, J. N., Kemner, C., Verbaten, M. N., & van Engeland, H. (2002). Gaze behavior of children with pervasive developmental disorder toward human faces: A fixation time study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 43, 669–678.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2008

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of Psychology, Neuroscience and BehaviourMcMaster UniversityHamiltonCanada

Personalised recommendations