Advertisement

Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders

, Volume 38, Issue 3, pp 574–580 | Cite as

Brief Report: Perception of Genuine and Posed Smiles by Individuals with Autism

  • Zillah L. BorastonEmail author
  • Ben Corden
  • Lynden K. Miles
  • David H. Skuse
  • Sarah-Jayne Blakemore
Brief Report

Abstract

Individuals with autism are impaired in the recognition of fear, which may be due to their reduced tendency to look at the eyes. Here we investigated another potential perceptual and social consequence of reduced eye fixation. The eye region of the face is critical for identifying genuine, or sincere, smiles. We therefore investigated this ability in adults with autism. We used eye-tracking to measure gaze behaviour to faces displaying posed and genuine smiles. Adults with autism were impaired on the posed/genuine smile task and looked at the eyes significantly less than did controls. Also, within the autism group, task performance correlated with social interaction ability. We conclude that reduced eye contact in autism leads to reduced ability to discriminate genuine from posed smiles with downstream effects on social interaction.

Keywords

Asperger syndrome Social cognition Face processing Smile Eye-tracking 

Notes

Acknowledgments

This study was funded by the Wellcome Trust and the Royal Society. ZB is funded by the Wellcome Trust Four Year PhD Programme in Neuroscience at UCL. SJB is funded by the Royal Society UK. We would like to thank Rebecca Chilvers for conducting the ADOS interviews for this study.

References

  1. Adolphs, R., Gosselin, F., Buchanan, T. W., Tranel, D., Schyns, P., & Damasio, A. R. (2005). A mechanism for impaired fear recognition after amygdala damage. Nature, 433, 68–72.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Adolphs, R., Sears, L., & Piven, J. (2001). Abnormal processing of social information from faces in autism. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 13, 232–240.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed.): Test revision (DSM-IV-TR). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.Google Scholar
  4. Back, E., Ropar, D., & Mitchell, P. (2007). Do the eyes have it? Inferring mental states from animated faces in autism. Child Development, 78, 397–411.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Dalton K. M., Nacewicz B. M., Alexander A. L., & Davidson, R. J. (2007). Gaze-fixation, brain activation, and amygdala volume in unaffected siblings of individuals with autism. Biological Psychiatry, 61(4), 512–520.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Dalton, K. M., Nacewicz, B. M., Johnstone, T., Schaefer, H. S., Gernsbacher, M. A., Goldsmith, H. H., et al. (2005). Gaze fixation and the neural circuitry of face processing in autism. Nature Neuroscience, 8, 519–526.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  7. Ekman, P. (1989). The argument and evidence about universals in facial expressions of emotion. In H. Wagner, & A. Manstead (Eds.), The biological psychology of emotions and social processes (pp. 143–164). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  8. Hager, J. C., & Ekman, P. (1985). The asymmetry of facial actions is inconsistent with models of hemispheric specialization. Psychophysiology, 22, 307–318.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Happe, F. G. E. (1993). Communicative competence and theory of mind in autism: A test of Relevance theory. Cognition, 48, 101–119.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Howard, M. A., Cowell, P. E., Boucher, J., Broks, P., Mayes, A., Farrant, A., et al. (2000). Convergent neuroanatomical and behavioural evidence of an amygdala hypothesis of autism. Neuroreport, 11, 2931–2935.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Joseph, R. M., & Tager-Flusberg, H. (2004). The relationship of theory of mind and executive functions to symptom type and severity in children with autism. Development and Psychopathology, 16, 137–155.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Klin, A., Jones, W., Schultz, R., Volkmar, F., & Cohen, D. (2002). Visual fixation patterns during viewing of naturalistic social situations as predictors of social competence in individuals with autism. Archives of General Psychiatry, 59, 809–816.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Klin, A., Sparrow, S. S., de Bildt, A., Cicchetti, D. V., Cohen, D. J., & Volkmar, F. R. (1999). A normed study of face recognition in autism and related disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 29, 499–508.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Langdell, T. (1978). Recognition of faces: an approach to the study of autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 19, 255–268.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Lord, C., Risi, S., Lambrecht, L., Cook, E. H., Jr., Leventhal, B. L., DiLavore, P. C., et al. (2000). The autism diagnostic observation schedule-generic: A standard measure of social and communication deficits associated with the spectrum of autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 30, 205–223.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Martin, I., & McDonald, S. (2004). An exploration of causes of non-literal language problems in individuals with Asperger Syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 34, 311–328.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Miles, L., & Johnston, L. (2006). Not all smiles are created equal: Perceiver sensitivity to the differences between posed and genuine smiles. In G. Haberman, & C. Flethcher-Flinn (Eds.), Cognition and language: Perspectives from New Zealand. Bowen Hills (Queensland, Australia): Australian Academic Press.Google Scholar
  18. Peace, V., Miles, L. K., & Johnston, L. (2006). It doesn’t matter what you wear: The impact of posed and genuine expressions of happiness on product evaluation. Social Cognition, 24, 137–168.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Pelphrey, K. A., Sasson, N. J., Reznick, J. S., Paul, G., Goldman, B. D., & Piven, J. (2002). Visual scanning of faces in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 32, 249–261.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Piven, J. (2001). The broad autism phenotype: A complementary strategy for molecular genetic studies of autism. American Journal of Medical Genetics, 105, 34–35.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Spezio M. L., Adolphs R., Hurley R. S., & Piven, J. (2007). Abnormal use of facial information in high-functioning autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37(5), 929–939.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Williams, L. M., Senior, C., David, A., Loughland, C. M., & Gordon, E. (2001). In search of the “Duchenne Smile”? evidence from eye movements. Journal of Psychophysiology, 15, 122–127.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Yin, R. K. (1969). Looking at upside-down faces. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 81, 141–145.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Yovel, G., & Kanwisher, N. (2005). The neural basis of the behavioural face-inversion effect. Current Biology, 15, 2256–2262.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2007

Authors and Affiliations

  • Zillah L. Boraston
    • 1
    Email author
  • Ben Corden
    • 1
  • Lynden K. Miles
    • 2
  • David H. Skuse
    • 1
  • Sarah-Jayne Blakemore
    • 3
  1. 1.Behavioural and Brain Sciences Unit, Institute of Child HealthUniversity College LondonLondonUK
  2. 2.University of CanterburyChristchurchNew Zealand
  3. 3.Institute of Cognitive NeuroscienceUniversity College LondonLondonUK

Personalised recommendations