Advertisement

Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders

, Volume 37, Issue 10, pp 1987–1995 | Cite as

Sibling Interaction of Children with Autism: Development Over 12 Months

  • Fiona KnottEmail author
  • Charlie Lewis
  • Tim Williams
Original Paper

Abstract

While deficits in social interaction are central to autism, the sibling relationship has been found to provide a key medium for the development of such skills. Naturalistic observations of sibling pairs including children with autism and controls with Down syndrome were made across two time periods, twelve months apart. Consistent with the evidence on typically developing children, the amount and rate of initiations of both prosocial and agonistic interaction increased, but further analysis suggested that these interactions were stage-managed by the typically developing children. Results show social interaction and imitation in children with autism and the special role that sibling interactions can play. Longitudinal research on the acquisition of social skills in children with developmental disabilities is needed.

Keywords

Autism Siblings Social interaction Longitudinal studies 

References

  1. Abramovitch, R., Corter, C., Pepler, D. J., & Stanhope, L. (1986). Sibling and peer interaction; a final follow up and comparison. Child Development, 57, 217–229.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Abramovitch, R., Stanhope, L., Pepler, D. J., & Corter, C. (1987). Influence of Down’s Syndrome on sibling interaction. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 28, 865–879. .PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Beadle-Brown, J., Murphy G., Wing, L., Gould, J., Shah, A., & Holmes, N. (2002). Changes in social impairment for people with intellectual disabilities: A follow-up of the Camberwell cohort. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 32, 195–206.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Beadle-Brown, J., & Whiten, A. (2004). Elicited imitation in children and adults with autism: is there a deficit? Journal of Intellectual and Developmental Disability, 29, 147–163.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Brown, J., & Whiten, A. (2000). Imitation, theory of mind and related activities in autism. An observational study of spontaneous behavior in everyday contexts. Autism, 4, 185–204.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Carpendale, J. I. M, & Lewis, C. (2004). Constructing an understanding of mind: The development of children’s social understanding within social interaction. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 27, 79–96.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  7. Carpendale, J. I. M., & Lewis, C. (2006). How children develop social understanding. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
  8. Dallas, E., Stevenson, J., & McGurk, H. (1993). Cerebral-palsied children’s interactions with siblings – II. Interactional structure. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 34, 649–671.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Dunn, J. (1988). Sibling influences on childhood development. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 29, 119–127.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Dunn, J., & Kendrick, C. (1979). Interaction between young siblings in the context of family relationships. In: M. Lewis, & L. Rosenblum (Eds.), The child and its family. New York:Plenum Press.Google Scholar
  11. Fein, D., Pennington, B., Markowitz, P., Braverman, M., & Waterhouse, L. (1986). Towards a neuropsychological model of infantile autism: are the social deficits primary? Journal of the American Academy of Child Psychiatry, 25, 198–212.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Frith, U. (2003). Autism. Oxford: Basil Blackwell.Google Scholar
  13. Hartmann, G. (1977). Considerations in the choice of interobserver reliability estimates. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis 10, 103–116.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Howlin, P. (1986). An overview of social behaviour in autism In E. Schopler, & G. Mesibov (Eds.), Social behaviour in autism. New York: Plenum Press.Google Scholar
  15. Howlin, P., Mawhood, L., & Rutter, M. (2000). Autism and developmental receptive language disorder - a follow-up comparison in early adult life. II: Social, behavioural, and psychiatric outcomes. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 41, 561–578.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Knott, F., Lewis, C., & Williams, T. (1995). Sibling interaction of children with learning disabilities: a comparison of autism and Down’s syndrome. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 36, 965–976.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. McGee, G., Feldman, R., & Morrier, M. (1997) Benchmarks of social treatment for children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 27, 353–364.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. McGovern, C., & Sigman, M. (2005). Continuity and change from early childhood to adolescence in autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 46, 401–408.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Meltzoff, A., & Decety, J. (2003). What imitation tells us about social cognition: a rapprochement between developmental psychology and cognitive neuroscience. Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London Series B – Biological Sciences, 358, 1431, 491–500.Google Scholar
  20. Meltzoff, A., & Moore, M. (2002). Imitation, memory, and the representation of persons. Infant Behavior and Development, 25, 39–61.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Perner, J., Ruffman, T., & Leekam, S.R. (1994). Theory of mind is contagious: You catch it from your sibs. Child Development, 65, 1228–1238.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Peterson, C.C. (2000). Kindred spirits: Influences of siblings’ perspectives on theory of mind. Cognitive Development, 15, 435–455.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Roeyers, H. (1996). The influence of non-handicapped peers on the social interactions of children with a pervasive developmental disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 26, 303–320.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Rogers, S. (2000). Interventions that facilitate socialization in children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 30, 399–408.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Rogers, S., Hepburn, S., Stackhouse, T., & Wehner, E. (2003). Imitation performance in toddlers with autism and those with other developmental disorders. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 44, 763–781.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Ruffman, T., Perner, J., Naito, M., Parkin, L., & Clements, W.A. (1998). Older (but not younger) siblings facilitate false belief understanding. Developmental Psychology, 34, 161–174.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Sigman, M., & Ruskin, E. (1999). Continuity and change in the social competence of children with autism, Down syndrome and developmental delays. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 64.Google Scholar
  28. Stoneman, Z. (2001). Supporting positive sibling relationships during childhood. Mental Retardation and Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews, 7, 134–142.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Stoneman, Z., Brody, G., Davis, C., & Crapps, J. (1989). Role relations between children who are mentally retarded and their older siblings: observations in three home contexts. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 10, 61–76.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Tomasello, M., Carpenter, M., Call, J., Behne, T., & Moll, H. (2005). Understanding and sharing intentions: The origins of cultural cognition. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 28, 675–691.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Volkmar, F., Carter, A., Grossman J., & Klin, A. (1997) Social development in autism. In D. Cohen, & F. Volkmar (Eds.), Handbook of autism and pervasive developmental disorders. New York: John Wiley and Sons.Google Scholar
  32. Williams, J., Whiten, A., & Sing, T (2004). A systematic review of action imitation in autistic spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 34, 285–299.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2007

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.School of Psychology and Clinical Language ScienceUniversity of ReadingReading, BerkshireUK
  2. 2.Psychology DepartmentLancaster University, Fylde CollegeLancasterUnited Kingdom

Personalised recommendations