Advertisement

Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders

, Volume 37, Issue 10, pp 2008–2013 | Cite as

Brief Report: Impaired Identification of Discrepancies Between Expressive Faces and Voices in Adults with Asperger’s Syndrome

  • Kate O’ConnorEmail author
Brief Report

Abstract

The aim of the present experiment was to examine the ability of adults with Asperger’s syndrome and age-matched typically-developing controls to identify incongruent and congruent emotional information from the face and voice. In the first part of the experiment, participants determined whether simultaneously presented expressive faces and voices were the same or different. In the second part, participants identified expressive faces and voices in isolation. Results showed that relative to controls, adults with AS were less accurate at distinguishing between congruent and incongruent expressive faces and voices. Both groups obtained similar accuracy to expressive faces and voices presented in isolation. These findings may partially explain some of the difficulties individuals on the autistic spectrum have with social interaction.

Keywords

Asperger’s syndrome Sensory integration Expression 

Notes

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by funding from the Ministry of Research, Science and Technology, New Zealand. I would like to thank the adults with AS and control subjects who participated in this study. Also thank you to Joss O’Connor for commenting on an earlier version of this manuscript and Dr Jeff Hamm for assistance with the signal detection analysis.

References

  1. Attwood, T. (1998). Asperger’s syndrome: A guide for parents and professionals (1st ed.). London: Jessica-Kingsley Publishers Ltd.Google Scholar
  2. APA. (1994). DSM-IV diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.Google Scholar
  3. Baron-Cohen, S., Golan, O., Wheelwright, S., & Hill, J. (2003). Mind reading emotions library. London: Jessica-Kingsley Publishers Ltd (www.jkp.com/mindreading).Google Scholar
  4. Birch, J. (2003). Congratulations! It’s Asperger’s syndrome. London: Jessica-Kingsley Publishers.Google Scholar
  5. de Gelder, B., & Vroomen, J. (2000). The perception of emotions by ear and eye. Cognition and Emotion, 14, 289–311.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. de Gelder, B., Vroomen, J., de Jong, S., Masthoff, E., Tromenaars, F., & Hodiamont, O. (2005). Multisensory integration of emotional faces and voices in schizophrenics. Schizophrenia Research, 72, 195–203.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Dolan, R., Morris, J., & de Gelder, B. (2001). Crossmodal binding of fear in voice and face. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 98(17), 10006–10010.Google Scholar
  8. Gillberg, C., & Gillberg, I. (1989). Asperger syndrome - some epidemiological considerations: A research note. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 30, 631–638.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Green, D., & Swets, J. (1966). Signal detection theory and psychophysics. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  10. Hall, G., Szechtman, H., & Nahmias, C. (2003). Enhanced salience and emotion recognition in autism: A PET study. American Journal of Psychiatry, 160(8), 1439–1441.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Happe, F., & Frith, U. (2006). The weak coherence account: Detail-focused cognitive style in autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36(1), 5–25.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Hobson, R. (1986). The autistic child’s appraisal of expressions of emotion. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 27(3), 321–342.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Hobson, R., Ouston, J., & Lee, A. (1988). Emotion recognition in autism: Coordinating faces and voices. Psychological Medicine, 18, 911–923.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Jackson, L., & Attwood, T. (2002). Freeks, geeks and Asperger’s syndrome. London: Jessica Kingsley.Google Scholar
  15. Loveland, K., Tunali-Kotoski, B., Chen, R., Brelsford, K., & Ortegon, J. (1995). Intermodal perception of affect by persons with Autism or Down syndrome. Development and Psychopathology, 7, 409–418.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. MacMillan, N., & Creelman, C. (1991). Detection theory: A user’s guide. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  17. McGurk, H., & McDonald, J. (1976). Hearing lips and seeing voices. Nature, 264, 746–748.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. McPartland, J., Dawson, G., Webb, S., Panagiotides, H., & Carver, L. (2004). Event-related brain potentials reveal anomalies in temporal processing of faces in autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45(7), 1235–1245.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Miller, J. (2003). Women from another planet?: Our lives in the Universe of autism. Milan: Dancing Mind Books.Google Scholar
  20. Mottron, L., Dawson, M., Soulieres, I., Hubert, B., & Burack, J. (2006). Enhanced perceptual functioning in autism: An update and eight principles of autistic perception. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36(1), 27–43.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. O’Connor, K., Hamm, J., & Kirk, I. (2005). The neurophysiological correlates of face processing in adults and children with Asperger’s syndrome. Brain and Cognition, 59(1), 82–95.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Plaisted, K., O’Riordan, M., & Baron-Cohen, S. (1998). Enhanced discrimination of novel, highly similar stimuli by adults with autism during a perceptual learning task. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 39, 765–775.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Sainsbury, C. (2000). Martian in the Playground. London: The Book Factory.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2007

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Psychology DepartmentUniversity of AucklandAucklandNew Zealand

Personalised recommendations