Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders

, Volume 37, Issue 10, pp 1823–1832 | Cite as

Empathizing and Systemizing in Adults with and without Autism Spectrum Conditions: Cross-Cultural Stability

  • Akio Wakabayashi
  • Simon Baron-Cohen
  • Tokio Uchiyama
  • Yuko Yoshida
  • Miho Kuroda
  • Sally Wheelwright
Original paper

Abstract

This study tests the empathizing-systemizing (E-S) theory of sex differences and the extreme male brain (EMB) theory of autism. Three groups of participants took part: n = 48 people with autism spectrum, n = 137 general population controls, and n = 1,250 university student controls. Each participant completed the Empathy Quotient (EQ) and the Systemizing Quotient (SQ). Results: The autism spectrum condition (ASC) group scored significantly lower than controls on the EQ, and significantly higher on the SQ. Among both control groups, females scored significantly higher than males on the EQ, whilst males scored significantly higher than females on the SQ. The distribution of ‘brain types’, based on the difference between EQ and SQ scores, showed distinct profiles for people with ASC, control males and control females.

Keywords

Empathizing Systemizing Autism Asperger syndrome Autism spectrum conditions Cognitive style Sex differences 

References

  1. American Psychiatric Association (1994). DSM-IV Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.Google Scholar
  2. Argyle, M., & Cooke, M. (1976). Gaze and mutual gaze. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  3. Baron-Cohen, S. (1995). Mindblindness: an essay on autism and theory of mind. Boston, MA: MIT Press/Bradford Books.Google Scholar
  4. Baron-Cohen, S. (1997). Are children with autism superior at folk physics? In H. Wellman, & K. Inagaki (Eds.), Children’s theories. New direction for child development series. Jossey-Bass Inc.Google Scholar
  5. Baron-Cohen, S. (2000). Theory of mind and autism: a fifteen year review. In S. Baron-Cohen, H. Tager-Flusberg, & D. Cohen (Eds.), Understanding other minds (pp. 3–20) 2nd edition. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  6. Baron-Cohen, S. (2002). The extreme male brain theory of autism. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6, 248–254.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Baron-Cohen, S. (2003). The essential difference: Men, women and the extreme male brain. London: Penguin.Google Scholar
  8. Baron-Cohen, S., & Bolton, P. (1993). Autism: The facts. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  9. Baron-Cohen, S., & Hammer, J. (1997). Is autism an extreme form of the male brain? Advances in Infancy Research, 11, 193–217.Google Scholar
  10. Baron-Cohen, S., & Wheelwright, S. (2004). The Empathizing Quotient (EQ): an investigation of adults with Asperger Syndrome and high-functioning autism, and normal sex differences. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 34, 163–175.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A. M., & Frith, U. (1985). Does the autistic child have a “theory of mind”? Cognition, 21, 37–46.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A. M., & Frith, U. (1986). Mechanical, behavioural and intentional understanding of picture stories in autistic children. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 4, 113–125.Google Scholar
  13. Baron-Cohen, S., Tager-Flusberg, H., & Cohen, D. J. (eds.), (1993). Understanding other minds: Perspectives from autism. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  14. Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., & Hill, J. (2001). The “Reading the Mind in the Eyes” Test, revised version: A study with normal adults, and adults with Asperger Syndrome or high-functioning autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 42, 241–252.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Baron-Cohen, S., Hoekstra, R., Knickmeyer, R., & Wheelwright, S. (in press). The Autism-Spectrum Quotient (AQ)—Children’s version. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders.Google Scholar
  16. Baron-Cohen, S., Jolliffe, T., Mortimore, C., & Robertson, M. (1997). Another advanced test of theory of mind: Evidence from very high functioning adults with autism or Asperger Syndrome. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 38, 813–822.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Stone, V., & Rutherford, M. (1999). A mathematician, a physicist, and a computer scientist with Asperger syndrome: Performance on folk psychology and folk physics test. Neurocase, 5, 475–483.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Baron-Cohen, S., O’Riordan, M., Jones, R., Stone, V., & Plaisted, K. (1999). A new test of social sensitivity: Detection of faux pas in normal children and children with Asperger syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 29, 407–418.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Baron-Cohen, S., Richler, J., Bisarya, D., Gurunathan, N., & Wheelwright, S. (2003). The systemizing quotient: an investigation of adults with Asperger Syndrome or high functioning autism, and normal sex differences. In U. Frith, & E. Hill (Eds.), Autism: mind and brain (pp. 161–186). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  20. Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Skinner, R., Martin, J., & Clubley, E. (2001). The Autism-Spectrum Quotient (AQ): Evidence from Asperger Syndrome/high-functioning autism, males and females, scientists and mathematicians. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 31, 5–17.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Spong, A., Scahill, V., & Lawson, J. (2001). Are intuitive physics and intuitive psychology independent? Journal of Development and Learning Disorders, 5, 47–78.Google Scholar
  22. Benbow, C. P., & Stanley, J. (1983). Sex differences in mathematical reasoning ability: More facts. Science, 222, 1029–1031.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Beneditt, J. (1994). Women in science ‘94. Comparisons across cultures. Science, 263, 1391–1396.Google Scholar
  24. Brush, S. G. (1991). Women in science and engineering. American Scientist, 79, 404–419.Google Scholar
  25. Buck, R. W., Savin, V. J., Miller, R. E., & Caul, W. F. (1972). Communication of affect through facial expression in humans. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 23, 362–371.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Charman, T., & Baron-Cohen, S. (1995). Understanding models, photos, and beliefs: A test of the modularity thesis of metarepresentation. Cognitive Development, 10, 287–298.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J. (1994). Origins of domain specificity: the evolution of functional organization. In L. A. Hirschfeld, & S. A Gelman (Eds.), Mapping the mind: domain specificity in cognition and culture (pp. 85–116). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  28. Davis, M. H. (1980). A multidimensional approach to individual differences in empathy. JSAS Catalog of Selected Documents in Psychology, 10, 85.Google Scholar
  29. Davis, M. H., & Franzoi, S. L. (1991). Stability and change in adolescent self-consciousness and empathy. Journal of Research in Personality, 25, 70–87.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Dennett, D. (1987). The intentional stance. Cambridge, Massachusetts: The MIT Press/Bradford Books.Google Scholar
  31. Geary, D. (1995). Sexual selection and sex differences in spatial cognition. Learning and Individual Differences, 7, 289–301.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Gillberg, C. (1992). The Emmanuel Miller Lecture, 1991: Autism and autistic-like conditions: Subclasses among disorders of empathy. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 33, 813–842.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Goldenfeld N., Baron-Cohen, S., & Wheelwright, S. (2005). Empathizing and systemizing in males, females, and autism. Clinical Neuropsychiatry, 2, 338–345.Google Scholar
  34. Hall, J. A. (1978). Gender effects in decoding nonverbal cues. Psychological Bulletin, 85, 845–858.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Halpern, D. (1992). Sex differences in cognitive ability. Hillsdale, NJ: Laurence Erlbaum Association.Google Scholar
  36. Happé, F. (1994). An advanced test of theory of mind: Understanding story characters’ thoughts and feelings by able autistic, mentally handicapped, and normal children and adults. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 24, 129–154.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Hermelin, B. (2002). Bright splinters of the mind: A personal story of research with autistic savants. London: Jessica kingsley.Google Scholar
  38. Hoffman, M. L. (1977). Sex differences in empathy and related behaviors. Psychological Bulletin, 84, 712–722.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Hofstede, G. (1980). Culture’s consequences: International differences in Work-Related Values. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
  40. Hofstede, G. (1991). Cultures and Organizations: Software of the mind. London: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
  41. Hui, C. H., & Triandis, H. C. (1986). Individualism-Collectivism: A study of cross-cultural researchers. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 17, 222–248.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Hyde, J., & Linn, M. (1988). Gender differences in verbal ability: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 104, 53–69.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Laungani, P. (2000). Culture influences on the development of identity: India and England. In J. Mohan (Eds.), Personality across cultures. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  44. Lawson, J., Baron-Cohen, S., & Wheelwright, S. (2004). Empathizing and systemizing in adults with and without Asperger Syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 34, 301–310.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Leekam, S., & Perner, J. (1991). Does the autistic child have a metarepresentational deficit? Cognition, 40, 203–218.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Leekam, S. R., Libby, S. J., Wing, L., Gould, J., & Taylor, C. (2002). The diagnostic interview for social and communication disorders: Algorithms for ICD−10 childhood autism and Wing and Gould autistic spectrum disorder. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 43, 327–342.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Leslie, A. M., & Thaiss, L. (1992). Domain specificity in conceptual development: Evidence from autism. Cognition, 43, 225–251.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Linn, M., & Petersen, A. (1985). Emergence and characterization of sex differences in spatial ability: A meta-analysis. Child Development, 56, 1479–1498.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Lummis, M., & Stevenson, H. (1990). Gender differences in beliefs and achievement: A cross-cultural study. Developmental Psychology, 26, 254–263.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Masters, M. S., & Sanders, B. (1993). Is the gender difference in mental rotation disappearing? Behavior Genetics, 23, 337–341.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Ounsted, C. & Taylor, D. (Eds.), (1972). Gender differences: Their ontogeny and significance. Churchill, Livingstone.Google Scholar
  52. Perner, J., Frith, U., Leslie, A. M., & Leekam, S. (1989). Exploration of the autistic child’s theory of mind: Knowledge, belief, and communication. Child Development, 60, 689–700.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Triandis, H. C. (1994). Culture and social behavior. New York: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
  54. Wakabayashi, A., Baron-Cohen, S., Sasaki, J., Ogawa, Y., & Wheelwright, S. (submitted). Are intuitive psychology and intuitive physics independent?: A study from the viewpoint of the Empathizing-Systemizing theory.Google Scholar
  55. Wechsler, D. (1981). Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale Revised edition. New York; The Psychological Corporation. (Japanese version: Shinagawa, F., Kobayashi, S.,Fujita, K., & Maekawa, H. (1990) Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale Revised version. Tokyo: Nihon Bunka Kagakusha.).Google Scholar
  56. Wellman, H. M. & Inagaki, K. (1997). The emergence of core domains of thought: Children’s reasoning about physical, psychological, and biological phenomena. Jossey-Bass Inc.Google Scholar
  57. Wheelwright, S., Baron-Cohen, S., Goldenfeld, N., Delaney, J., Fine, D., Smith, R., Weil, L., & Wakabayashi, A. (2006). Predicting Autism Spectrum Quotient (AQ) from the Systemizing Quotient—Revised (SQ-R) and Empathy Quotient (EQ). Brain Research, 1079, 47–56.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  58. Wing, L., Leekam, S. R., Libby, S. J., Gould, J., & Larcombe, M. (2002). The diagnostic interview for social and communication disorders: Background, inter-rater reliability and clinical use. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 43, 307–325.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  59. Witkin, H., Oltman, P., Raskin, E., & Karp, S. (1971). A manual for the Embedded Figures Test. Consulting psychologists Press.Google Scholar
  60. World Health Organization (1992). International classification of diseases (10th ed.). Geneva: World Health Organization.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2006

Authors and Affiliations

  • Akio Wakabayashi
    • 1
    • 2
  • Simon Baron-Cohen
    • 2
  • Tokio Uchiyama
    • 3
    • 4
  • Yuko Yoshida
    • 4
  • Miho Kuroda
    • 4
  • Sally Wheelwright
    • 2
  1. 1.Department of PsychologyChiba UniversityInageJapan
  2. 2.Autism Research Centre, Department of PsychiatryUniversity of Cambridge CambridgeUK
  3. 3.Department of Human Relations, Faculty of Human WelfareOtsuma Women’s UniversityTama, TokyoJapan
  4. 4.Psycho-Developmental ClinicTsuzukiJapan

Personalised recommendations