Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders

, Volume 37, Issue 9, pp 1761–1775

The Problem with Using Eye-Gaze to Infer Desire: A Deficit of Cue Inference in Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder?

Original Paper

Abstract

Children with autism respond atypically to eye-gaze cues, arguably because they fail to understand that eye-gaze conveys mentalistic information. Three experiments investigated whether a difficulty in inferring desire from eye-gaze in autism reflects a failure to understand the mentalistic significance of eye-gaze, an inhibitory deficit or a deficit of cue inference. While there was an inhibitory component to the tasks, children with autism were no more affected by this than controls. In addition, individuals’ impairment in inferring desire was not limited to social cues, but was also observed when desire was cued by more general cues. Consequently, children with autism may have a general deficit in using arbitrary cues to make inferences, which impacts particularly on their social development.

Keywords

Autism spectrum disorder Desire inference Eye-gaze Arrow Cue inference 

References

  1. Anderson, J. R., & Doherty, M. J. (1997). Preschoolers’ perception of other people’s looking: Photographs and drawings. Perception, 26, 333–343.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Baron-Cohen, S. (1995). Mindblindness: An essay on autism and theory of mind. Cambridge: The MIT.Google Scholar
  3. Baron-Cohen, S., Campbell, R., Karmiloff-Smith, A., Grant, J., & Walker, J. (1995). Are children with autism blind to the mentalistic significance of the eyes? British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 13, 379–398.Google Scholar
  4. Bayliss, D. M., Jarrold, C., Gunn, D. M., & Baddeley, A. D. (2003). The complexities of complex span: Explaining individual differences in working memory in children and adults. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 132, 71–92.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Carlson, S. M., Davis, A. C., & Leach, J. G. (2005). Less is more. Executive function and symbolic representation in preschool children. Psychological Science, 16, 609–616.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Carlson, S. M., Mandell, D. J., & Williams, L. (2004). Executive function and theory of mind: Stability and prediction from ages 2 to 3. Developmental Psychology, 40, 1105–1122.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Carlson, S. M., & Moses, L. J. (2001). Individual differences in inhibitory control and children’s theory of mind. Child Development, 72, 1032–1053.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Charman, T. (2003). Why is joint attention a pivotal skill in autism? Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B, 358, 315–324.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Charman, T., Baron-Cohen, S., Swettenham, J., Baird, G., Cox, A., & Drew, A. (2000). Testing joint attention, imitation, and play as infancy precursors to language and theory of mind. Cognitive Development, 15, 481–498.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Chawarska, K., Klin, A., & Volkmar, F. (2003). Automatic attention cueing through eye movements in 2 year old children with autism. Child Development, 74, 1108–1122.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Diamond, A., Kirkham, N., & Amso, D. (2002). Conditions under which young children can hold two rules in mind and inhibit a prepotent response. Developmental Psychology, 38, 352–362.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Driver, J., Davis, G., Ricciardelli, P., Kidd, P., Maxwell, E., & Baron-Cohen, S. (1999). Gaze perception triggers reflexive visuospatial orienting. Visual Cognition, 6(5), 509–540.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Dunn, L. M., Dunn, L. M., Whetton, C., & Burley, J. (1997). British picture vocabulary scale (2nd ed.). Windsor, UK: NFER-Nelson.Google Scholar
  14. Einav, S., & Hood, B. M. (2006). Children’s use of the temporal dimension of gaze for inferring preference. Developmental Psychology, 42, 142–152.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Farroni, T., Johnson, M. H., Brockbank, M., & Simion, F. (2000). Infants’ use of eye-gaze direction to cue attention: The importance of perceived motion. Visual Cognition, 7, 705–718.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Flynn, E., O’Malley, C., & Wood, D. (2004). A longitudinal, microgenetic study of the emergence of false belief understanding and inhibition skills. Developmental Science, 7, 103–115.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Grelotti, D. J., Gauthier, I., & Schultz, R. (2002). Social interest and the development of cortical face specialization: What autism teaches us about face processing. Developmental Psychobiology, 40, 213–225.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Hala, S., & Russell, J. (2001). Executive control within strategic deception: A window on early cognitive development? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 80, 112–141.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Hill, E. L. (2004). Executive dysfunction in autism. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8, 26–32.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Hughes, C. (1998). Executive function in preschoolers: Links with theory of mind and verbal ability. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 16, 233–253.Google Scholar
  21. Hughes, C., & Russell, J. (1993). Autistic children’s difficulty with mental disengagement from an object: Its implications for theories of autism. Developmental Psychology, 29, 498–510.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Jarrold, C., Boucher, J., & Russell, J. (1997). Language profiles in children with autsim. Autism, 1, 57–76.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Joseph, R. M., & Tager-Flusberg, H. (2004). The relationship of theory of mind and executive functions to symptom type and severity in children with autism. Development and Psychopathology, 16, 137–155.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Kjelgaard, M. M., & Tager-Flusberg, H. (2001). An investigation of language impairment in autism: Implications for genetic subgroups. Language and Cognitive Processes, 16, 287–308.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Kylliäinen, A., & Hietanen, J. K. (2004). Attention orienting by another’s gaze direction in children with autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45, 435–444.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Lee, K., Eskritt, M., Symons, L. A., & Muir, D. (1998). Children’s use of triadic eye gaze information for “mind reading”. Developmental Psychology, 34, 525–539.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Leekam, S. R., Lopez, B., & Moore, C. (2000). Attention and joint attention in preschool children with autism. Developmental Psychology, 36, 261–273.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Mecklenbräuker, S., Hupbach, A., & Wippich, W. (2001). What colour is the car? Implicit memory for colour information in children. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 54A, 1069–1086.Google Scholar
  29. Miller, G. A., & Chapman, J. P. (2001). Misunderstanding analysis of covariance. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 110, 40–48.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Minshew, N. J., Meyer, J., & Goldstein, G. (2002). Abstract reasoning in autism: A dissociation between concept formation and concept identification. Neuropsychology, 16, 327–334.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Ozonoff, S., Pennington, B. F., & Rogers, S. J. (1991). Executive function deficits in high-functioning autistic individuals—relationship to theory of mind. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 32, 1081–1105.Google Scholar
  32. Pellicano, E., & Rhodes, G. (2003). The role of eye-gaze in understanding other minds. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 21, 33–43.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Pennington, B. F., & Ozonoff, S. (1996). Executive functions and developmental psychopathology. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 37, 51–87.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Potter, M. (1967). On perceptual recognition. In J. S. Bruner R. R. Olver, & P. M. Greenfield (Eds.), Studies in cognitive growth (pp. 103–134). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  35. Raven, J. C., Court, J. H., & Raven, J. (1990). Coloured progressive matrices. Oxford: Oxford Psychologists Press.Google Scholar
  36. Repacholi, B. M., & Gopnik, A. (1997). Early reasoning about desires: Evidence from 14- and 18-months olds. Developmental Psychology, 3, 12–21.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Ristic, J., Mottron, L., Kelland Friesen, C., Iarocci, G., Burack, J. A., & Kingstone, A. (2005). Eyes are special but not for everyone: The case of autism. Cognitive Brain Research, 24, 715–718.Google Scholar
  38. Russell, J. (1997). Autism as an executive disorder. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  39. Russell, J., Mauthner, N., Sharpe, S., & Tidswell, T. (1991). The windows task as a measure of strategic deception in preschoolers and autistic subjects. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 9, 331–349.Google Scholar
  40. Rutter, M., Bailey, A., & Lord, C. (2003). The social communication questionnaire. Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services.Google Scholar
  41. Senju, A., Tojo, Y., Dairoku, H., & Hasegawa, T. (2004). Reflexive orienting in response to eye gaze and an arrow in children with and without autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45, 445–458.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Simpson, A., & Riggs, K. J. (2005). Factors responsible for performance on the day–night task: Response set or semantics? Developmental Science, 8(4), 360–371.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Swettenham, J., Condie, S., Campbell, R., Milne, E., & Coleman, M. (2003). Does the perception of moving eyes trigger reflexive visual orienting in autism? Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B, 358, 325–334.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Tomasello, M., Carpenter, M., Call, J., Behne, T., & Moll, H. (2005). Understanding and sharing intentions: The origins of cultural cognition. Behavioural and Brain Sciences, 28, 675–735.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Triesch, J., Teuscher, C., Deák, G. O., & Carlson, E. (2006). Gaze following: Why (not) learn it? Developmental Science, 9, 125–157.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Vlamings, P. H. J. M., Stauder, J. E. A., van Son, I. A. M., & Mottron, L. (2005). Atypical visual orienting to gaze- and arrow-cues in adults with high functioning autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 35, 267–276.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Wing, L., & Gould, J. (1979). Severe impairments of social interaction and associated abnormalities in children: Epidemiology and classification. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 9, 11–29.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2006

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of Experimental PsychologyUniversity of BristolBristolUK

Personalised recommendations