Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders

, Volume 37, Issue 6, pp 1086–1095

Emotion Perception in Asperger’s Syndrome and High-functioning Autism: The Importance of Diagnostic Criteria and Cue Intensity

Original Paper

Abstract

This study compared emotion perception accuracy between children with Asperger’s syndrome (AS) and high-functioning autism (HFA). Thirty children were diagnosed with AS or HFA based on empirically supported diagnostic criteria and administered an emotion perception test consisting of facial expressions and tone of voice cues that varied in intensity. Participants with AS and the typically developing standardization sample of the emotion perception instrument had the same mean emotion perception accuracy, whereas participants with HFA performed significantly worse. Results also provided preliminary evidence for a difference in accuracy perceiving low-intensity tone of voice cues between participants with HFA and AS. Future research to build on these initial findings should include attention to tone of voice, underlying processing, and cue intensity.

Keywords

Emotion Nonverbal cues Asperger’s syndrome High-functioning autism Diagnosis 

References

  1. Achenbach, T. M., & Rescorla, L. A. (2001). Manual for the ASEBA school-age forms and profiles. Burlington, VT: University of Vermont, Research Center for Children, Youth, & Famlies.Google Scholar
  2. Adolphs, R., Sears, L., & Piven, J. (2001). Abnormal processing of social information from faces in autism. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 13, 232–240.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. American Psychiatric Association (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders– (4th ed Text Revision). Washington, DC: Author.Google Scholar
  4. Asperger, H. (1944). Die “Autistischen Psychopathen” im Kindesalter. Archiv fur Psychiatrie und Nervenkrankheiten, 117, 76–136.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., & Jolliffe, T. (1997). Is there a “Language of the Eyes”? Evidence from normal adults, and adults with autism or Asperger syndrome. Visual Cognition, 4, 311–331.Google Scholar
  6. Baum, K. M., & Nowicki, S. (1998). Perception of emotion: Measuring decoding accuracy of adult prosodic cues varying intensity. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 89, 89–107.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Braverman, M., Fein, D., Lucci, D., & Waterhouse, L. (1989). Affect comprehension in children with pervasive developmental disorders. Journal of Autism & Developmental Disorders, 19, 301–316.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Brown, J. R., & Dunn, J. (1996). Continuities in emotion understanding from three to six years. Child Development, 67, 789–802.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Bruininks, R. H., Woodcock, R. W., Weatherman, R. F., & Hill, B. K. (1996). Scales of independent behavior-revised: comprehensive manual. Chicago, IL: Riverside Publishing Company.Google Scholar
  10. Cohen, J., Cohen, P., West, S. G., & Aiken, L. S. (2003). Applied multiple regression/correlation analysis for the behavioral sciences (3rd ed.). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Publishers.Google Scholar
  11. Denham, S. A. (1998). Emotional development in young children. New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  12. Deruelle, C., Rondan, C., Gepner, B., & Tardif, C. (2004). Spatial frequency and face processing in children with autism and Asperger syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 34, 199–210. .PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Eckman, P., Friesen, W. V., & Ellsworth, P. (1972). Emotion in the human face. New York: Pergamon Press.Google Scholar
  14. Fein, D., Lucci, D., Braverman, M., & Waterhouse, L. (1992). Comprehension of affect in context in children with Pervasive Developmental Disorders. Journal of Child Psychiatry and Psychology, 33, 1157–1167.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Hobson, R. P. (1986a). The autistic child’s appraisal of expressions of emotion. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 27, 321–342.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Hobson, R. P. (1986b). The autistic child’s appraisal of expressions of emotion: A further study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 27, 671–680.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Hobson, R. P, Ouston, J., & Lee, A. (1989). Naming emotion in faces and voices: Abilities and disabilities in autism and mental retardation. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 7, 237–250.Google Scholar
  18. Izard, C. E. (1972). The face of emotion. New York: Appelton-Century-Crofts.Google Scholar
  19. Izard, C. E. (1977). Human emotions. New York: Plenum Press.Google Scholar
  20. Klin, A., Jones, W., Schultz, R., Volkmar, F., & Cohen, D. (2002). Defining and quantifying the social phenotype in autism. American Journal of Psychiatry, 159, 895–908.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Klin, A., Pauls, D., Schultz, R., & Volkmar, F. (2005). Three diagnostic approaches to Asperger syndrome: Implications for research. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 35, 221–234.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Koning, C., & Magill-Evans, J. (2001). Social and language skills in adolescent boys with Asperger syndrome. Autism, 5, 23–36.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Lord, C., Storoschuck, S., Rutter, M., & Pickles, A. (1993). Using the ADI-R to diagnose autism in preschool children. Infant Mental Health Journal, 14, 234–252.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Lord, C., Risi, S., Lambrecht, L., Cook, E. H., Leventhal, B. L., et al. (2000). The Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule-Generic: A standard measure of social and communication deficits associated with the spectrum of autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 30, 205–223.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Lord, C., Rutter, M., & Couteur, A. L. (1994). Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised: A revised version of the diagnostic interview for caregivers of individuals with possible pervasive developmental disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 24, 659–685.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Macintosh, K. E., & Dissanayake, C. (2004). Annotation: The similarities and differences between autistic disorder and Asperger’s disorder: A review of the empirical evidence. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45, 421–434.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Macdonald, H., Rutter, M., Howlin, P., Rios, P., Le Conteur, A., Evered, C., & Folstein, S. (1989). Recognition and expression of emotional cues by autistic and normal adults. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 30, 865–877.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Mattron, L. (2004). Matching strategies in cognitive research with individuals with high-functioning autism: Current practices, instrument biases, and recommendations. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 34, 19–27.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Miller, G. M. (2001). Misunderstanding analysis of covariance. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 110, 40–48.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Nijiokiktjien, C., Verschoor, A., de Sonneville, L., Huyser, C., Op het Veld, V., Toorenaar, N. (2001). Disordered recognition of facial identity and emotions in three Asperger type autists. European Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 10, 79–90.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Nowicki, S. (2003). Manual for the receptive tests of the diagnosticanalysis of nonverbal accuracy 2. Unpublished manual.Google Scholar
  32. Nowicki, S., & Carton, J. (1993). The measurement of emotional intensity from facial expressions. The Journal of Social Psychology, 133, 749–750.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Nowicki, S., & Duke, M. P. (1992). The association of children’s nonverbal decoding abilities with their popularity, locus of control, and academic achievement. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 153, 385–391.Google Scholar
  34. Ross, D. C., & Klein, D. F. (1988). Group mating: Is this a research technique to be avoided? Educational and Psychological Measurement, 48, 281–295.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Schopler, E., Mesibov, G. B., & Kunce, L. J. (1998). Asperger syndrome or HFA. Current Issues in Autism Series. New York: Plenum Press.Google Scholar
  36. Serra, M., Minderaa, R. B., van Geert, P. L. C., & Jackson, A. E. (1999). Social-cognitive abilities in children with lesser variants of autism: Skill deficits or failure to apply skills? European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 8, 301–311.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Smith-Myles, B., & Simpson, R. L. (2002). Asperger syndrome: An overview of characteristics. Focus on Autism and Other Developmental Disabilities, 17, 132–137.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Tantam, D., Monaghan, L., Nicholson, H., & Stirling, J. (1989). Autistic children’s ability to interpret faces: A research note. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 30, 623–630.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Volkmar, F. R., & Klin, A. (1996). Yale special interests survey. Unpublished questionnaire.Google Scholar
  40. Volkmar, F. R., Klin, A., Schultz, R. T., Rubin, E., & Bronen, R. (2000). Asperger’s disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry, 157, 262–267.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Wechsler, D. (1991). Manual for the Wechsler intelligence scale for children (3rd ed.). San Antonio: The Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
  42. Wing, L. (1981). Asperger’s syndrome: A clinical account. Psychological Medicine, 11, 115–129.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2006

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of PsychologyVirginia Commonwealth UniversityRichmondUSA
  2. 2.Department of PsychiatryVirginia Commonwealth UniversityRichmondUSA
  3. 3.Departments of Pediatrics and PsychiatryUniversity of PittsburghPittsburghUSA
  4. 4.Child Development UnitChildren’s Hospital of PittsburghPittsburghUSA

Personalised recommendations