Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders

, Volume 36, Issue 5, pp 665–675

Discrimination in Autism Within Different Sensory Modalities

Original paper
  • 1k Downloads

Abstract

Recent studies have suggested that unusual visual processing in autism might stem from enhanced visual discrimination. Although there are also many anecdotal reports of auditory and tactile processing disturbances in autism these have received comparatively little attention. It is possible that the enhanced discrimination ability in vision in autism might extend to other modalities and further that they may underlie many reports of unusual touch and audition. The present study investigated the performance of children with and without autism on auditory and tactile discrimination tasks and revealed superior auditory but comparable tactile discrimination in autism relative to controls. These results extend previous findings of perceptual discrimination in autism and may be relevant for a neuro-developmental hypothesis of the disorder.

Keywords

Autism Discrimination Auditory Tactile 

References

  1. APA. (1994). DSM-IV diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed.). Washington DC: American Psychiatric Association.Google Scholar
  2. Baranek, G. T., Foster, L. G., & Berkson, G. (1997). Sensory defensiveness in persons with developmental disabilities. Occupational Therapy Journal of Research, 17, 173–185.Google Scholar
  3. Bettison, S. (1996). The long-term effects of auditory training on children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 27(3), 347–348.Google Scholar
  4. Blackstock, E. G. (1978). Cerebral Asymmetry and the development of early infantile autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 8, 339–353.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Bruneau, N., Roux, S., Adrien, J. L., & Barthélémy, C. (1999). Auditory associative cortex dysfunction in children with autism: Evidence from late auditory evoked potentials (N1 wave-T complex). Clinical Neurophysiology, 110, 1927–1934.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Bryson, S. E., Wainwright-Sharpe, J. A., & Smith, I. M. (1990). Autism: A developmental neglect syndrome? In J. T. Enns (Ed.), The development of attention: Research and theory. Amsterdam: North-Holland.Google Scholar
  7. Chess, S. (1977). Follow-up report on autism in congenital rubella. Journal of Autism and Childhood Schizophrenia, 7, 69–81.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Collet, L., Roge, B., Descouens, D., Moron, P., Duverdy, F., & Urgell, H. (1993). Objective auditory dysfunction in infantile autism. Lancet, 342, 923–924.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Courchesne, E., Kilman, B. A., Galambos, R., & Lincoln, A. J. (1984). Autism – processing of novel auditory information assessed by event-related brain potentials. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 59(3), 238–248.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Dahlgren, S. O., & Gillberg, C. (1989). Symptoms in the 1st 2 years of life – a preliminary population study of infantile-autism. European Archives Of Psychiatry and Neurological Sciences, 238(3), 169–174.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Dawson, G., Finley, C., Phillips, S., Galpert, L., & Lewy, A. (1988). Reduced P3 amplitude of the event-related brain potential: Its relationship to language ability in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 18, 493–504.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Dawson, G., Meltzoff, A. N., Osterling, J., Rinaldi, J., & Brown, E. (1998). Children with autism fail to orient to naturally occurring social stimuli. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 28, 479–485.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Duncan, J., & Humphreys, G. W. (1989). Visual-search and stimulus similarity. Psychological Review, 96(3), 433–458.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Frith, U. (1989). Autism: Explaining the enigma, Oxford: Basil Blackwell.Google Scholar
  15. Geldard, F. A. (1972). The human senses (2nd ed.). New York: John Wiley and Sons Inc.Google Scholar
  16. Gillberg, C. (1990). Autism and pervasive developmental disorders. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 31(1), 99–119.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Gillberg, C., Ehlers, S., Schaumann, H., Jacobsson, G., Dahlgren, S. O., Lindblom, R., Bagenholm, A., Tjuust, T., & Blidner, E. (1990). Autism under age three years: A clinical study of 28 cases referred for autistic symptoms in infancy. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 31, 921–934.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Gottlieb, G. (1971). Ontogenesis of sensory function in birds and mammals. In E. Tobach, L. R. Aronson & E. F. Shaw (Eds.), The biopsychology of development (pp. 67–128). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  19. Grandin, T. (1992a). Calming effects of deep touch pressure in patients with autistic disorder, college students and animals. Journal of Child and Adolescent Psychopharmacology, 2, 63–72.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Grandin, T. (1992b). An inside view of autism. In E. Schopler & G. Mesibov (Eds.), High-functioning individuals with autism. New York: Plenum Press.Google Scholar
  21. Grandin, T. (1997). A personal perspective on autism. In D. J. Cohen & F. R. Volkmar (Eds.), Handbook of autism and pervasive developmental disorders (2nd ed.). New York: John Wiley and Sons, Inc.Google Scholar
  22. Gwiazda, J., & Birch, E. E. (2001). Perceptual development: Vision. In E. B. Goldstein (Ed.), Handbook of perception. Blackwell.Google Scholar
  23. Hainline, L. (1998). How the visual system develops: Normal and abnormal development. In A. Slater (Ed.), Perceptual development: Visual, auditory and speech perception in infancy. Hove, Sussex: Psychology Press Ltd.Google Scholar
  24. Hainline, L., & Abramov, I. (1992). Assessing visual development: Is infant vision good enough? In C. Rovee-Collier & L. P. Lipsitt, (Eds.), Advances in infancy research. New Jersey: Norwood.Google Scholar
  25. Hayes, R. (1987). Training for work. In D. J. Cohen & A. M. Donnellan (Eds.), Handbook of autism and pervasive developmental disorders. Silver Springs, MD: Winston.Google Scholar
  26. Heaton, P., Hermelin, B., & Pring, L. (1998). Autism and pitch processing: A precursor for savant musical ability? Music Perception, 15(3), 291–305.Google Scholar
  27. Heller, M. A. (1991). Introduction. In M. A. Heller & W. Schiff, (Eds.), The Psychology of Touch (pp. 1–19). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
  28. Iverson, P., & Kuhl, P. K. (1995). Mapping the peceptual magnet effect for speech using signal detection theory and multidimensional scaling. Journal of Acoustic Society of America, 97, 553–562.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Jolliffe, T., & Baron-Cohen, S. (1997). Are people with autism or Asperger’s Syndrome faster than normal on the Embedded Figures Task? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 38, 527–534.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Kanner, L. (1943). Autistic disturbance of affective contact. Nervous Child, 2, 217–250.Google Scholar
  31. Khalfa, S., Bruneau, N., Roge, B., Georgieff, N., Veuillet, E., Adrien, J. L., Barthélémy, C., & Collet, L. (2001). Peripheral auditory asymmetry in infantile autism. European Journal of Neuroscience, 13(3), 628–632.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Kootz, J. P., Marinelli, B., & Cohen, D. J. (1980). Sensory receptor sensitivity in autistic children: Response times to proximal and distal stimuli. Archives of General Psychiatry, 38, 271–273.Google Scholar
  33. Kuhl, P. (1991). Human adults and human infants show the perceptual magnet effect for the prototypes of speech categories, monkeys doe not. Perception and Psychophysics, 50, 93–107.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  34. Lincoln, A. J., Courchesne, E., Harms, L., & Allen, M. (1995). Sensory modulation of auditory stimuli in children with autism and receptive developmental language disorder: Event-related brain potential evidence. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 25, 521–539.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Lockyer, L., & Rutter, M. (1969). A five to fifteen year follow-up study of infantile psychosis, III. British Journal of Psychiatry, 115, 865–882.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Lord, C., Rutter, M., & Le Couteur, A. (1994). Autism diagnostic interview-revised: A revised version of a diagnostic interview for caregivers of individuals with possible pervasive developmental disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 24, 659–685.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. MacMillan, N.A., & Creelman, C.D. (2004). Detection theory: a user’s guide (2nd ed.). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
  38. Mari, M., Castiello, U., Marks, D., Marraffa, C., & Prior, M. (2003).The reach-to-grasp movement in children with autism spectrum disorder. The Philosophical Transactions of The Royal Society: Biological Sciences, 358, 393–403.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Minshew, N. J. (1996). Brief report: Brain mechanisms in autism: Functional and structural abnormalities. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 26(2), 205–209.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Minshew, N. J., Goldstein, G., & Siegel, D. J. (1997). Neuropsychologic functioning in autism: Profile of a complex information processing disorder. Journal of International Neuropsychological Society, 3, 303–316.Google Scholar
  41. Mottron, L., & Burack, J. A. (2001). Enhanced Perceptual Functioning in the Development of Autism. In J. A. Burack, T. Charman, N. Yirmiya & P. R. Zelazo (Eds.), The development of autism: Perspectives from theory and research. New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
  42. Mottron, L., Peretz, I., & Menard, E. (2000). Local and global processing of music in high-functioning persons with autism: Beyond central coherence? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 41(8), 1057–1065.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Muller, R. A., Behen, M. E., Rothermel, R. D., Chugani, D. C., Muzik, O., Mangner, T. J., & Chugani, H. T. (1999). Brain mapping of language and auditory perception in high-functioning autistic adults: A PET study. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 29, 19–31.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. NASC. (1978). National society for autistic children definition of the syndrome of autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 8, 162–167.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. O’Neill, M., & Jones, R. S. P. (1997). Sensory-perceptual abnormalities in autism: A case for more research. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 27(3), 283–293.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. O’Riordan, M., Plaisted, K., Driver, J., & Baron-Cohen, S. (2001). Superior visual search in autism. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27(3), 719–730.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. O’Riordan, M. A. (2000). Superior target detection in autism does not result from differential levels of target excitation or distractor inhibition. Cognition, 77, 81–96.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. O’Riordan, M. A., & Plaisted, K. C. (2001). Enhanced discrimination in autism. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 54A(4), 961–979.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Ornitz, E. M. (1974). The modulation of sensory input and motor output in autistic children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 4, 197–215.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Plaisted, K., O’Riordan, M., & Baron-Cohen, S. (1998a). Enhanced discrimination of novel, highly similar stimuli by adults with autism during a perceptual learning task. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 39, 765–775.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Plaisted, K., O’Riordan, M., & Baron-Cohen, S. (1998b). Enhanced visual search for a conjunctive target in autism: A research note. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 39, 777–783.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Plaisted, K. C. & O’Riordan, M. A. (in preparation). Autistic children show enhanced perceptual learning – further evidence of reduced generalisation in autism.Google Scholar
  53. Rapin, I. (1997). Autism. New England Journal Of Medicine, 337(2), 97–104.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Raven, J., Court, J., & Raven, J. (1992). The standard progressive matrices, Oxford: Oxford Psychology Press.Google Scholar
  55. Rosenhall, U., Nordin, V., Sandstrom, M., Ahlsen, G., & Gillberg, C. (1999). Autism and hearing loss. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 29, 349–366.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. Rumsey, J., & Hamberger, S. (1988). Neuropsychological findings in high functioning men with infantile autism, residual state. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 10, 201–221.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  57. Russell, J. (Ed.) (1997). Autism as an executive disorder. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  58. Schopler, E. (1965). Early infantile autism and receptor processes. Archives of General Psychiatry, 13, 327–335.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  59. Shah, A., & Frith, U. (1983). An islet of ability in autism: A research note. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 24, 613–620.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  60. Shah, A., & Frith, U. (1993). Why do autistic individuals show superior performance on the block design test? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 34, 1351–1364.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  61. Thivierge, J., Bedard, C., Cote, R., & Maziade, M. (1990). Brain-stem auditory evoked-response and subcortical abnormalities in autism. American Journal Of Psychiatry, 147(12), 1609–1613.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  62. Turkewitz, G., & Kenney, P. (1982). Limitations on input as a basis for neural organisation and perceptual development: A preliminary theoretical statement. Developmental Psych-biology, 15, 357–368.Google Scholar
  63. Weisenberger, J. M. (1991). Curtaneous perception. In E. B. Goldstein (Ed.), Handbook of Perception (pp. 535–566). Blackwell.Google Scholar
  64. Werker, J. F., Gilbert, J. H. V., Humphrey, K., & Tees, R. C. (1981). Developmental aspects of cross-language speech perception. Child Development, 52, 349–355.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  65. Werner, L. A., & Bernstein, I. L. (2001). Development of audition, somesthesis, taste & olfaction. In E. B. Goldstein (Ed.), Handbook of perception (pp. 670–708). Blackwell.Google Scholar
  66. Wolfe, J. M., Cave, K. R., & Franzel, S. L. (1989). A modified feature integration model for visual search. Journal of experimental psychology: Human perception and performance, 15, 419–433.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media, Inc. 2006

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.University of CambridgeCambridgeUK
  2. 2.Autism Research Centre, Department of Developmental PsychiatryUniversity of CambridgeCambridgeUK

Personalised recommendations