Skin Conductance Responses to Another Person’s Gaze in Children with Autism

Article

Abstract

The effects of another person’s gaze on physiological arousal were investigated by measuring skin conductance responses (SCR). Twelve able children with autism and 12 control children were shown face stimuli with straight gaze (eye contact) or averted gaze on a computer monitor. In children with autism, the responses to straight gaze were stronger than responses to averted gaze, whereas there was no difference in the responses to these gaze conditions in normally developing children. Thus, these results showed that eye gaze elicited differential pattern of SCR in normally developing children and in children with autism. It is possible that the enhanced arousal to eye contact may contribute to the abnormal gaze behaviour frequently reported in the context of autism.

KEY WORDS:

High functioning autism skin conductance responses electrodermal activity eye contact gaze direction 

References

  1. American Psychiatric Association (1994) Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4). Washington DC: AuthorGoogle Scholar
  2. Andreassi J. L. (2000). Psychophysiology: Human behavior & physiological response (4). New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum AssociateGoogle Scholar
  3. Anstis S. M., Mayhew J. W., Morley T., (1969). The perception of where a face or television “portrait” is looking American Journal of Psychology 82: 474–489PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Argyle M., (1975). Body communication. London: Richard Clay LtdGoogle Scholar
  5. Baron-Cohen S., Baldwin D. A., Crowson M., (1997) Do children with autism use the speaker’s direction of gaze strategy to crack the code of language? Child Development 68: 48–57PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Baron-Cohen S., Campbell R., Karmiloff-Smith A., Grant J., Walker J., (1995) Are children with autism blind to the mentalistic significance of the eyes? British Journal of Developmental Psychology 13: 379–398Google Scholar
  7. Baron-Cohen S., Wheelwright S., Jolliffe T., (1997). Is there “language of the eyes”? Evidence from normal adults, and adults with autism or Asperger syndrome Visual Cognition 4: 311–331CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Barry R. J., James A. L., (1988). Coding of stimulus parameters in autistic, retarded, and normal children: Evidence for two-factor theory of autism International Journal of Psychophysiology 6: 136–149Google Scholar
  9. Batki A., Baron-Cohen S., Wheelwright S., Connellan J., Ahluwalia J., (2000). Is there an innate gaze module? Evidence from human neonates Infant Behavior & Development 23: 223–229CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Blair R. J. R., (1999). Psychophysiological responsiveness to the distress of others in children with autism Personality and Individual Differences 23: 477–485CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Buitelaar J. K., van Engeland H., De Kogel K. H., De Vries H., van Hooff J. A. R. A. M., (1991). Differences in the structure of social behaviour of autistic children and non-autistic retarded controls Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 32: 995–1015PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Caron A. J., Caron R., Roberts J., Brooks R., (1997). Infant sensitivity to deviations in dynamic facial-vocal displays: The role of eye regard Developmental Psychology 33: 802–813CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  13. Chawarska K., Klin A., Volkmar F., (2003). Automatic attention cueing through eye movement in 2-year-old children with autism Child Development 74: 1108–1122CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  14. Clark-Carter D., (1997). Doing quantitative psychological research: From design to report. East Sussex: Psychology Press LtdGoogle Scholar
  15. Cline M. G., (1967). The perception of where a person is looking American Journal of Psychology 80: 41–50PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Dawson M., Schell A., Fillon D., (1990). The electrodermal system. In: Cacioppo J. T., Tassinary L. G., (Eds.), Principles of psychophysiology New York: Cambridge University Press. pp. 295–324Google Scholar
  17. Detenber B. H., Simons R. F., Bennett G. G., (1998). Roll’em: the effects of picture motion on emotional responses Journal of Broadcasting and Electronic Media 42: 112–126Google Scholar
  18. Donovan W. L., Leavitt L. A., (1980). Physiologic correlates of direct and averted gaze Biological Psychology 10: 189–199CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  19. Engeland H. van (1984). The electrodermal orienting response to auditive stimuli in autistic children, normal children, mentally retarded children, and child psychiatric patients Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 14: 261–279PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Engeland H. van, Roelofs J. W., Verbaten M. N., Slangen J. L., (1991). Abnormal electrodermal reactivity to novel visual stimuli in autistic children Psychiatry Research 38: 27–38CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  21. Farroni T., Csibra G., Simion F., Johnson M. H., (2002). Eye contact detection in humans from birth Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 99: 9602–9605CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  22. Gale A., Spratt G., Chapman A. J., Smallbone A., (1975). EEG correlates of eye contact and interpersonal distance Biological Psychology 3: 237–245CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  23. Gibson J. J., Pick A. D., (1963) Perception of another person’s looking behavior American Journal of Psychology 76: 386–394PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Hains S. M. J., Muir D. W., (1996). Infant sensitivity to adult eye direction Child Development 67: 1940–1951PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Hirstein W., Iversen P., Ramachandran V. S., (2001). Autonomic responses of autistic children to people and objects Proceedings of Royal Society London B 268: 1883–1888CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Howard M. A., Cowell P. E., Boucher J., Broks P., Mayes A., Farrant A., Roberts N., (2000). Convergent neuroanatomical and behavioural evidence of an amygdale hypothesis of autism Neuroreport 11: 2931–2935PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Hutt C., Ounsted C., (1966). The biological significance of gaze aversion with particular reference to the syndrome of infantile autism Behavioral Science 11: 346–356PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Jaffe J., Stern P. N., Perry J. C., (1973). “Conversational” coupling of gaze behavior in prelinguistic human development Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 2: 321–330CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  29. James A. L., Barry R. J., (1984). Cardiovascular and electrodermal responses to simple stimuli in autistic, retarded and normal children International Journal of Psychophysiology 1: 179–193CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  30. Jonides J., Mack R., (1984). On the cost and benefit of cost and benefit Psychological Bulletin 96: 29–44CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Joseph R. M., Tanaka J., (2003). Holistic and part-based face recognition in children with autism Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 44: 529–542PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Kanner L., (1943). Autistic disturbances of affective contact Nervous Child 2: 217–250Google Scholar
  33. Kasari C., Sigman M., Yirmiya N., (1993) Focused and social attention of autistic children in interactions with familiar and unfamiliar adults: A comparison of autistic, mentally retarded, and normal childrenDevelopment and Psychopathology 5: 403–414Google Scholar
  34. Kleinke C. L., (1986). Gaze and eye contact: A research review Psychological Bulletin 100: 78–100CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  35. Kleinke C. L., Pohlen P. D., (1971). Affective and emotional responses as a function of other person’s gaze and cooperativeness in a two-person game Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 17: 308–313PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Klin A., Jones W., Schultz R., Volkmar F., Cohen D., (2002). Visual fixation patterns during viewing of naturalistic social situations as predictors of social competence in individuals with autism Archives of General Psychiatry 59: 809–816CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  37. Kylliäinen A., Hietanen J. K., (2004). Attention orienting by another’s gaze direction in children with autism Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 44: 435–444CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Leavitt L. A., Donovan W. L., (1979). Perceived infant temperament, locus of control, and maternal physiological response to infant gaze Journal of Research in Personality 13: 267–278CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Leekam S., Baron-Cohen S., Perrett D., Milders M., Brown S., (1997). Eye-direction detection: A dissociation between geometric and joint attention skills in autism British Journal of Developmental Psychology 15: 77–95Google Scholar
  40. Lord C., Rutter M., Le Couteur A., (1994). Autism diagnostic interview-revised: A revised version of diagnostic interview for caregivers of individuals with possible pervasive developmental disorders Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 24: 659–685CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  41. Masame K., (1990). Perception of where a person is looking: Overestimation and underestimation of gaze direction Tohoku Psychologica Folia 49: 33–41Google Scholar
  42. Maurer D., (1985). Infants’ perception of facedness. In: Field T. M., Fox N. A., (Eds.), Social Percepton in infants. New Jersey: Norwood pp. 73–100Google Scholar
  43. McBride G., King M. G., James J. W., (1965). Social proximity effects on galvanic skin responses in adult humans The Journal of Psychology 61: 153–157PubMedGoogle Scholar
  44. Mirenda P. L., Donnellan A. M., Yoder D. E., (1983). Gaze behavior: A new look at an old problem Journal of Autism and Develpomental Disorders 13: 397–409CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Nicholas K. A., Champness B. G., (1971). Eye gaze and the GSR Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 7: 623–626CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Osterling J., Dawson G., (1994). Early recognition of children with autism: A study of first birthday home videotapes Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 24: 247–257CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  47. Palkovitz R. J., Wiesenfeld A. R., (1980). Differential autonomic responses of autistic and normal children Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 10: 347–361PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Pederson J., Livoir-Petersen M. F., Schelde J. T., (1989). An ethological approach to autism: An analysis of visual behaviour and interpersonal contact in a child versus adult interaction Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica 80: 346–355PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Pelphrey K. A., Sasson N. J., Reznick J. S., Paul G., Goldman B. D., Piven J. (2002). Visual scanning of faces in autism Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 32: 249–261CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  50. Phillips W., Baron-Cohen S., Rutter M., (1992). The role of eye contact in goal detection: Evidence from normal infants and children with autism or mental handicap Development and Psychopathology 4: 375–383Google Scholar
  51. Senju A., Tojo Y., Dairoku H., Hasegawa T., (2004). Reflexive orienting in response to eye gaze and an arrow in children with and without autism Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 44: 445–458CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Senju A., Yaguchi K., Tojo Y., Hasegawa T., (2003). Eye contact does not facilitate detection in children with autism Cognition 89: B43–B51CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  53. Simons R. F., Detenber B. H., Roedema T. M., Reiss J. E., (1999). Emotion processing in three systems: The medium and the message Psychophysiology 36: 619–627CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  54. Stevens S., Gruzelier J., (1984). Electrodermal activity to auditory stimuli in autistic, retarded, and normal children Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 14: 245–260PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Swettenham J., Baron-Cohen S., Charman T., Cox A., Braid G., Drew A., Rees L., Wheelwright S., (1998). The frequency and distribution of spontaneous attention shifts between social and nonsocial stimuli in autistic, typically developing, and nonautistic developmentally delayed infants Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 39: 747–753CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  56. Swettenham J., Condie S., Campbell R., Milne E., Coleman M., (2003). Does the perception of moving eyes trigger reflexive visual orienting in autism? Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London: Biological 358: 325–334CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  57. Tan J., Harris P., (1991). Autistic children understand seeing and wanting Development and psychopathology 3: 163–174CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  58. Tantam D., Holmes D., Cordess C., (1993). Nonverbal expression in autism of Asperger type Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 23: 111–133CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  59. Tinbergen N., (1974). Ethology and stress disseasesScience 5: 367–377Google Scholar
  60. Vecera S. P., Johnson M. H., (1995). Gaze detection and the cortical processing of faces: Evidence from infants and adults Visual Cognition 2: 59–87CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  61. Volkmar F. R., Mayers L. C., (1990). Gaze behavior in autism Development and Psychopathology 2: 61–69Google Scholar
  62. Wade M. W., Jones R. F., (1982). The accuracy of eye-gaze judgement: A signal detection approach British Journal of Social Psychology 21: 293–299PubMedGoogle Scholar
  63. Willemsen-Swinkles S. H. N., Buitelaar J. K., Weijnen F. G., van Engeland H., (1998). Timing of social gaze behavior in children with a pervasive developmental disorder Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 28: 199–210CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media, Inc. 2006

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.University of TampereTampereFinland
  2. 2.Department of PsychologyUniversity of TampereTampereFinland

Personalised recommendations