Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders

, Volume 35, Issue 4, pp 429–443 | Cite as

How do Individuals with Asperger Syndrome Respond to Nonliteral Language and Inappropriate Requests in Computer-mediated Communication?

  • Gnanathusharan Rajendran
  • Peter Mitchell
  • Hugh Rickards
Article

Abstract

Computer-mediated communication in individuals with Asperger syndrome, Tourette syndrome and normal controls was explored with a program called Bubble Dialogue (Gray, Creighton, McMahon, & Cunninghamn (1991)) in which the users type text into speech bubbles. Two scenarios, based on Happé (1994) were adapted to investigate understanding of figure of speech and sarcasm, and a third, developed by ourselves, looked at responses to inappropriate requests (lending money and disclosing home address on a first meeting). Dialogue transcripts were assessed by 62 raters who were blind to the clinical diagnoses. Hierarchical linear modelling revealed that rated understanding of a figure of speech was predicted mainly by verbal ability and executive ability, as well as by clinical diagnosis, whereas handling inappropriate requests was predicted by age, verbal ability, executive ability and diagnosis. Notably, the Tourette comparison group showed better understanding than the Asperger group in interpreting a figure of speech and handling inappropriate requests, and differences between these groups were possibly attributable to individual differences in executive ability. In contrast, understanding sarcasm was predicted by age but not by either verbal ability, executive ability or clinical diagnosis. Evidently, there is a complicated relation between Asperger syndrome, verbal ability and executive abilities with respect to communicative performance.

Keywords

Asperger syndrome communication Bubble Dialogue role-play 

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed. text revised). Washington, DCGoogle Scholar
  2. Baddeley, A., Wilson, B. A. 1988Frontal amnesia and the dysexecutive syndromeBrain and Cognition7212230CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  3. Baker, M.J., Koegel, R.L., Kogel, L.K. 1998Increasing the social behaviour of young children with autism using their obsessive behavioursJournal of the Association for Persons with Severe Handicaps23300308Google Scholar
  4. Baron-Cohen, S. (1995). Mindblindness. Cambridge, MA: MIT PressGoogle Scholar
  5. Baron-Cohen, S., O’Riordan, M., Stone, V., Jones, R., Plaisted, K. 1999Recognition of faux pas by normally developing children and children with Asperger syndrome or High-Functioning AutismJournal of Autism and Developmental Disorders29407418CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  6. Bishop, D. V. M., Norbury, C. F. 2005aExecutive functions in children with communication impairments, in relation to autistic symptomatology - 1: GenerativityAutism9727CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Bishop, D. V. M., Norbury, C. F. 2005bExecutive functions in children with communication impairments, in relation to autistic symptomatology - 2: Response inhibitionAutism92943CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Bornstein, R. 1990Neuropsychological performance in children with Tourette syndromePsychiatry Research337381CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  9. Bornstein, R. 1991Neuropsychological correlates of obsessive characteristics in Tourette syndromeJournal of Neuropsychiatry3157163Google Scholar
  10. Bryk, A. S., & Raudenbush, S. W. (1992). Hierarchical linear models for social and behavioral research: Application and data analysis models. Newbury Park, CA: SageGoogle Scholar
  11. Bryk, A. S., & Raudenbush, S. W. (2002). Hierarchical linear models for social and behavioral research: Application and data analysis models (2nd ed.). Newbury Park, CA: SageGoogle Scholar
  12. Bryk, A. S., Raudenbush, S. W., & Cogden, R. (2001). Hierarchical linear modeling with HLM 5 programs (version 5.04) [computer program manual]. Chicago. Illinois: Scientific Software InternationalGoogle Scholar
  13. Channon, S., Charman, T., Heap, J., Crawford, S., Rios, P. 2001Real-life type problem solving in Asperger’s syndromeJournal of Autism and Developmental Disorders31461469CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  14. Davidson, G. (1996). Chambers guide to grammar and usage. St Ives: CambersGoogle Scholar
  15. Demorest, A., Meyer, C., Phelps, E., Gardener, H., Winner, E. 1984Words speak louder than actions: Understanding deliberately false remarksChild Development5515271534Google Scholar
  16. Evans, J. J., Chua, S. E., McKenna, P. J., Wilson, B. A. 1997Assessment of the dysexecutive syndrome in schizophreniaPsychological Medicine27635646CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  17. Fine, C., Lumsden, J., Blair, R. J. R. 2001Dissociation between ‘theory of mind’ and executive functions in a patient with early left amygdala damageBrain124287298CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  18. Frith, U. (1989). Autism: Explaining the enigma. Oxford: BlackwellGoogle Scholar
  19. Frith, U. (2003). Autism: Explaining the enigma (2nd ed.). Oxford: BlackwellGoogle Scholar
  20. Frye, D., Zelazo, P. D., Palfai, T. 1995Theory of mind and rule-based reasoningCognitive Development10483527CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Gibbs, R. W. 1986On the psycholinguistics of sarcasmJournal of Experimental Psychology: General115315CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Gibbs, R. W. (1994). The poetics of mind. Figurative thought, language, and understanding. Cambridge: Cambridge University PressGoogle Scholar
  23. Gillman, J. E., Carter, A. S., Volkmar, F. R., Sparrow, S. S. 2000Towards a developmental operational definition of autismJournal of Autism and Developmental Disorders30269278CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  24. Gray, B., Creighton, N., McMahon, M., & Cunningham, D. (1991). Getting started with Bubble Dialogue, Language Development and HyperMedia Research Group Internal Report, University of Ulster at Coleraine, UlsterGoogle Scholar
  25. Green, J., Gilchrist, A., Burton, D., Cox, A. 2000Social and psychiatric functioning in adolescents with Asperger syndrome compared with conduct disorderJournal of Autism and Developmental Disorders30279293CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  26. Hadwin, J., Baron-Cohen, S., Howlin, P., Hill, K. 1996Can we teach children with autism to understand emotions, belief, or pretence?Development and Psychopathology8345365Google Scholar
  27. Happé, F. G. E. 1994An advanced test of theory of mind: Understanding of story characters’ thoughts and feelings by able autistic, mentally handicapped and normal children and adultsJournal of Autism and Developmental Disorders24129154PubMedGoogle Scholar
  28. Happé, F. G. E. 1995The role of age and verbal ability in the theory of mind task performance of subjects with autismChild Development66843855Google Scholar
  29. Jolliffe, T., Baron-Cohen, S. 1999The Strange Stories test: A replication of with high-functioning adults with autism or Asperger syndromeJournal of Autism and Developmental Disorders29395406CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  30. Jorgensen, J., Miller, G., Sperber, D. 1984A test of the mention theory of ironyJournal of Experimental Psychology: General1019Google Scholar
  31. Klin, A. 2000Attributing social meaning to ambiguous visual stimuli in higher-functioning autism and Asperger syndrome: The social attribution taskJournal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry41831846CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  32. Kreuz, R. J., Glucksberg, S. 1989How to Be Sarcastic: The Echoic Reminder Theory of Verbal IronyJournal of Experimental Psychology: General118373386Google Scholar
  33. Liss, M., Fein, D., Allen, D., Dunn, M., Feinstein, C., Morris, R., Waterhouse Rapin, L. I. 2001Executive functioning in high-functioning children with autismJournal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry42261270CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  34. Loveland, K. A., Pearson, D. A., Tunali-Kotoski, B., Ortegon, J., Cullen Gibbs, M. 2001Judgements of Social Appropriateness by Children and Adolescents with AutismJournal of Autism and Developmental Disorders31367376CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  35. Mitchell, P., Saltmarsh, R., Russell, H. 1997Overly literal interpretations of speech in autism: Apprehending the mind behind the messageJournal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry38685692PubMedGoogle Scholar
  36. Norris, G., Tate, R. L. 2000The Behavioural Assessment of the Dysexecutive Syndrome (BADS): Ecological, Concurrent and Construct validityNeuropsychological Rehabilitation103345CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. O’Neill, M., Jones, R. S. P. 1997Sensory-perceptual abnormalities in Autism: A case for more research?Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders27283293CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  38. Parsons, S., Mitchell, P. 2002The potential of virtual reality in social skills training for people with autistic spectrum disordersJournal of Intellectual Disability Research46430443CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  39. Prior, M., Eisenmajer, R., Leekam, S., Wing, L., Gould, G., Ong, B., Dowe, D. 1998Are there subgroups within the autistic spectrum? A cluster analysis of a group of children with autistic spectrum disordersJournal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry39893902CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  40. Rajendran, G., Mitchell, P. 2000Computer mediated interaction in Asperger’s syndrome: The Bubble Dialogue programComputers and Education35187207CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Rickards, H. 1995Tics and fits. The current status of Gilles de al Tourette syndrome and its relationship with epilepsySeizure4259266CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  42. Riggs, K. J., Peterson, D. M., Robinson, E. J., Mitchell, P. 1998Are errors in false belief tasks symptomatic of a broader difficulty with counterfactuality?Cognitive Development137390CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Roberston, M., Trimble, M. R., Lees,  1988The psychopathology of the Gilles de la Tourette syndrome: A phenomenological analysisBritish Journal of Psychiatry152383390PubMedGoogle Scholar
  44. Steiger, H., Gauvin, L., Jabalpurwala, S., Séguin, J. R., Stotland, S. 1999Hypersensitivity to social interactions in bulimic syndromes: Relationship to binge eatingJournal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology67765775CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  45. Stuss, D. T., Gallup, G. G., Alexander, M .P. 2001The frontal lobes are necessary for ‘theory of mind’Brain124279286CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  46. Swettenham, J. S. 1996Can Children with autism be taught to understand false belief using computers?Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry37157165PubMedGoogle Scholar
  47. Turner, M. A. 1999Generating novel ideas: Fluency performance in high-functioning and learning disabled individuals with autismJournal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry40189201CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  48. Wechsler, D. (1999). Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence (WASI). The Psychological CorporationGoogle Scholar
  49. Wilson, B. A., Alderman, N., Burgess, P. W., Emslie, H. C., & Evans, J. J. (1996). The Behavioural Assessment of the Dysexecutive Syndrome. Thames Valley Test Company: Flempton, Bury St. EdmundsGoogle Scholar
  50. Wing, L., Gould, J. 1979Severe impairments of social interaction and associated abnormalities in children: Epidemiology and classificationJournal of Autism and Developmental Disorders91129CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  51. Winner, E., Rosenstiel, A. K., Gardner, H. 1976The development of metaphoric understandingDevelopmental Psychology12289297CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Woolfe, T., Want, S. C., Siegal, M. 2002Signposts to development: Theory of mind in deaf childrenChild Development73768778CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media, Inc. 2005

Authors and Affiliations

  • Gnanathusharan Rajendran
    • 1
  • Peter Mitchell
    • 2
  • Hugh Rickards
    • 3
  1. 1.School of EducationThe University of EdinburghEdinburghUK
  2. 2.School of PsychologyThe University of NottinghamUniversity Park, NottinghamUK
  3. 3.The Queen Elizabeth Psychiatric HospitalEdgbaston, BirminghamUK

Personalised recommendations