Advertisement

Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology

, Volume 44, Issue 8, pp 1439–1453 | Cite as

Associations between Infant Behaviors during the Face-To-Face Still-Face Paradigm and Oppositional Defiant and Callous-Unemotional Behaviors in Early Childhood

  • Nicholas J. Wagner
  • W. Roger Mills-Koonce
  • Cathi B. Propper
  • Michael T. Willoughby
  • Pete D. Rehder
  • Ginger A. Moore
  • Martha J. Cox
Article

Abstract

Deficits in social orienting (i.e., gazing toward caregivers) during dyadic interactions and reactivity to stressful stimuli have been identified as behavioral correlates of oppositional defiant disorder (ODD) and callous-unemotional (CU) behaviors in older children. The goal of the current study was to investigate infants’ mother-directed gaze and reactivity during the face-to-face and still-face episodes of the face-to-face stillface paradigm performed at 6 months in the prediction of ODD and CU behaviors in early childhood. Using data from the Durham Child Health and Development study (n = 206), hierarchical regression analyses revealed that infants’ negative reactivity during the still-face episode and mother-directed gaze during the face-to-face episode predicted fewer ODD behaviors in early childhood. Examination of interaction effects suggested that mother-directed gaze attenuated the negative relation between reactivity and ODD and CU behaviors in early childhood. The current study is one of the first to extend downward the investigation of ODD and CU behaviors into infancy.

Keywords

Oppositional defiant Callous-unemotional Infancy Reactivity Parenting 

Notes

Acknowledgments

We thank all the parents and children who participated in the Durham Child Health and Development Study and the research assistants for their valuable efforts in collecting this data.

Compliance with Ethical Standards

Funding

This study was supported by the National Science Foundation through Children’s Research Initiative Grant (BCS-0126475).

Conflict of Interest

Nicholas Wagner declares that he has no conflict of interest. Roger Mills-Koonce declares that he has no conflict of interest. Cathi Propper declares that she has no conflict of interest. Michael Willoughby declares that he has no conflict of interest. Pete Rehder declares that he has no conflict of interest. Ginger Moore declares that she has no conflict of interest. Martha Cox declares that she has no conflict of interest.

Ethical Approval

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.

Informed Consent

Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.

References

  1. Achenbach, T. M. & Rescorla, L. (2000). ASEBA preschool forms & profiles: An integrated system of multi-informant assessment. Burlington: ASEBA.Google Scholar
  2. Adamson, L. B. & Frick, J. E. (2003). The still face: a history of a shared experimental paradigm. Infancy, 4, 451–473. doi: 10.1207/S15327078IN0404_01.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Ainsworth, M., Blehar, M. C., Waters, E., & Wall, S. (1978). Patterns of attachment: A psychological study of the strange situation. Oxford England: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  4. Barker, E. D., Oliver, B. R., Viding, E., Salekin, R. T., & Maughan, B. (2011). The impact of prenatal maternal risk, fearless temperament and early parenting on adolescent callous unemotional traits: a 14-year longitudinal investigation. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 52, 878–888. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2011.02397.x.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  5. Bedford, R., Pickles, A., Sharp, H., Wright, N., & Hill, J. (2014). Archival report reduced face preference in infancy: a developmental precursor to callous-unemotional traits? Biological Psychiatry, 78,1–7. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2014.09.022.
  6. Blair, R. J. R. (2008). Fine cuts of empathy and the amygdala: dissociable deficits in psychopathy and autism. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 61, 157–170.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Blair, J., Mitchell, D., & Blair, K. (2005). The psychopath: Emotion and the brain. Malden: Blackwell Publishing.Google Scholar
  8. Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment and loss v. 3 (vol. Vol. 1, ). London: Random House.Google Scholar
  9. Bradley, R. H. & Corwyn, R. F. (2008). Infant temperament, parenting, and externalizing behavior in first grade: a test of the differential susceptibility hypothesis. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 49, 124–131.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  10. Burke, J. D., Rowe, R., & Boylan, K. (2014). Functional outcomes of child and adolescent oppositional defiant disorder symptoms in young adult men. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 55, 264–272. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12150.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  11. Butterworth, G. (1991). The ontogeny and phylogeny of joint visual attention. In A. Whiten (Ed.), Natural theories of mind: Evolution, development and simulation of everyday mindreading (pp. 223–232). Cambridge: Basil Blackwell.Google Scholar
  12. Campbell, S. B. (2002). Behavior problems in preschool children: Clinical and developmental issues (2nd ed.). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  13. Campbell, S. B., Pierce, E. W., Moore, G., Marakovitz, S., & Newby, K. (1996). Boys’ externalizing problems at elementary school age: pathways from early behavior problems, maternal control, and family stress. Development and Psychopathology, 8, 701–719.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Cicchetti, D. & Dawson, G. (2002). Editorial: multiple levels of analysis. Development and Psychopathology, 14, 417–420. doi: 10.1017/S0954579402003012.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  15. Cohn, J. F., Campbell, S. B., & Ross, S. (1991). Infant response in the still-face paradigm at 6 months predicts avoidant and secure attachment at 12 months. Development and Psychopathology, 3, 367–376. doi: 10.1017/S0954579400007574.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Colder, C. R., Mott, J. A., & Berman, A. S. (2002). The interactive effects of infant activity level and fear on growth trajectories of early childhood behavior problems. Development and Psychopathology, 14, 1–23.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  17. Dadds, M. R., El Masry, Y., Wimalaweera, S., & Guastella, A. J. (2008). Reduced eye gaze explains ‘fear blindness’ in childhood psychopathic traits. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 47, 455–463. doi: 10.1097/CHI.0b013e31816407f1.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  18. Dadds, M. R., Jambrak, J., Pasalich, D., Hawes, D. J., & Brennan, J. (2011). Impaired attention to the eyes of attachment figures and the developmental origins of psychopathy. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 52, 238–245. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2010.02323.x.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  19. Dadds, M. R., Allen, J. L., Oliver, B. R., Faulkner, N., Legge, K., Moul, C., et al. (2012). Love, eye contact and the developmental origins of empathy v. psychopathy. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 200, 191–196. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.110.085720.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  20. Dadds, M. R., Allen, J. L., McGregor, K., Woolgar, M., Viding, E., & Scott, S. (2014). Callous-unemotional traits in children and mechanisms of impaired eye contact during expressions of love: a treatment target? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 55, 771–780. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12155.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  21. Deater-Deckard, K. & Dodge, K. A. (1997). Externalizing behavior problems and discipline revisited: nonlinear effects and variation by culture, context, and gender. Psychological Inquiry, 8, 161–175. doi: 10.1207/s15327965pli0803_1.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Dietrich, A., Riese, H., Sondeijker, F. L., Greaves-Lord, K., van Roon, A. M., Ormel, J., et al. (2007). Externalizing and internalizing problems in relation to autonomic function: a population-based study in preadolescents. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 46, 378–386. doi: 10.1097/CHI.0b013e31802b91ea.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  23. Ekas, N. V., Haltigan, J. D., & Messinger, D. S. (2013). The dynamic still-face effect: do infants decrease bidding over time when parents are not responsive? Developmental Psychology, 49, 1027–1035. doi: 10.1037/a0029330.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  24. Fearon, R., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., van IJzendoorn, M. H., Lapsley, A., & Roisman, G. I. (2010). The significance of insecure attachment and disorganization in the development of children s externalizing behavior: a meta-analytic study. Child Development, 81, 435–456. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01405.x.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  25. Frick, P. J. & Morris, A. (2004). Temperament and developmental pathways to conduct problems. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 33, 54–68. doi: 10.1207/S15374424JCCP3301_6.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  26. Frick, P. J. & White, S. F. (2008). Research review: the importance of callous-unemotional traits for developmental models of aggressive and antisocial behavior. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 49, 359–375. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2007.01862.x.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  27. Frick, P. J., Lahey, B. B., Loeber, R., Tannenbaum, L., Van Horn, Y., Christ, M. A. G., et al. (1993). Oppositional defiant disorder and conduct disorder: a meta-analytic review of factor analyses and cross-validation in a clinic sample. Clinical Psychology Review, 13, 319–340. doi: 10.1016/0272-7358(93)90016-F.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Frick, P. J., Cornell, A. H., Bodin, S., Dane, H. E., Barry, C. T., & Loney, B. R. (2003). Callous unemotional traits and developmental pathways to severe conduct problems. Developmental Psychology, 39, 246–260. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.39.2.246.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  29. Frick, P. J., Ray, J. V., Thornton, L. C., & Kahn, R. E. (2014). Can callous-unemotional traits enhance the understanding, diagnosis, and treatment of serious conduct problems in children and adolescents? A comprehensive review. Psychological Bulletin, 140, 1–57. doi: 10.1037/a0033076.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  30. Gilliom, M. & Shaw, D. S. (2004). Co-development of externalizing and internalizing problems in early childhood. Development and Psychopathology, 16, 313–333.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  31. Gottlieb, G. (1992). Individual development and evolution: The genesis of novel behavior. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  32. Hare, R. D., Hart, S. D., & Harpur, T. J. (1991). Psychopathy and the DSM-IV criteria for antisocial personality disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 100, 391–398. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.100.3.391.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  33. Hawes, D. J. & Dadds, M. R. (2007). Stability and malleability of callous-unemotional traits during treatment for childhood conduct problems. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 36, 347–355. doi: 10.1080/15374410701444298.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  34. Hipwell, A. E., Pardini, D., Loeber, R., Sembower, M., Keenan, K., & Stouthamer-Loeber, M. (2007). Callous-unemotional behaviors in young girls: shared and unique effects relative to conduct problems. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 36, 293–304.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  35. Hyde, L. W., Shaw, D. S., Gardner, F., Cheong, J., Dishion, T. J., & Wilson, M. (2013). Dimensions of callousness in early childhood: links to problem behavior and family intervention effectiveness. Development and Psychopathology, 25, 347–363. doi: 10.1017/S0954579412001101.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  36. Kagan, J. (1994). Galen’s prophecy: Temperament in human nature. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
  37. Kiehl, K. A., Smith, A. M., Hare, R. D., Mendrek, A., Forster, B. B., Brink, J., et al. (2001). Limbic abnormalities in affective processing by criminal psychopaths as revealed by functional magnetic resonance imaging. Biological Psychiatry, 50, 677–684. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3223(01)01222-7.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  38. Kimonis, E. R., Fanti, K., Anastassiou-Hadjicharalambous, X., Mertan, B., Goulter, N., & Katsimicha, E. (2015). Can callous-unemotional traits be reliably measured in preschoolers? Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. doi: 10.1007/s10802-015-0075-y.Google Scholar
  39. Kochanska, G. (1997). Mutually responsive orientation between mothers and their young children: implications for early socialization. Child Development, 68, 94–112. doi: 10.2307/1131928.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  40. Kochanska, G., Aksan, N., Knaack, A., & Rhines, H. M. (2004). Maternal parenting and children's conscience: early security as moderator. Child Development, 75, 1229–1242. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00735.x.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  41. Kochanska, G., Forman, D. R., Aksan, N., & Dunbar, S. B. (2005). Pathways to conscience: early mother-child mutually responsive orientation and children's moral emotion, conduct, and cognition. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 46, 19–34. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00348.x.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  42. Kochanska, G., Barry, R. A., Stellern, S. A., & O'Bleness, J. J. (2009). Early attachment organization moderates the parent–child mutually coercive pathway to children’s antisocial conduct. Child Development, 80, 1288–1300. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01332.x.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  43. Loeber, R. & Hay, D. (1997). Key issues in the development of aggression and violence from childhood to early adulthood. Annual Review of Psychology, 48, 371–410. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.48.1.371.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  44. Loney, B. R., Lima, E. N., & Butler, M. A. (2006). Trait affectivity and nonreferred adolescent conduct problems. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 35, 329–336. doi: 10.1207/s15374424jccp3502_17.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  45. Lorber, M. F., Del Vecchio, T., & Slep, A. M. S. (2014). The emergence and evolution of infant externalizing behavior. Development and Psychopathology, 27,1–18. doi: 10.1017/S0954579414000923.
  46. Lykken, D. T. (1995). The antisocial personalities. Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc..Google Scholar
  47. Lynam, D. R., Caspi, A., Moffitt, T. E., Loeber, R., & Stouthamer-Loeber, M. (2007). Longitudinal evidence that psychopathy scores in early adolescence predict adult psychopathy. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 116, 155–165. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.116.1.155.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  48. Maccoby, E. E. (1992). The role of parents in the socialization of children: an historical overview. Developmental Psychology, 28, 1006–1017. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.28.6.1006.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Meredith, W. & Tisak, J. (1990). Latent curve analysis. Psychometrika, 55, 107–122. doi: 10.1007/BF02294746.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Mesman, J., van IJzendoorn, M. H., & Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J. (2009). The many faces of the still-face paradigm: a review and meta-analysis. Developmental Review, 29, 120–162. doi: 10.1016/j.dr.2009.02.001.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Mills‐Koonce, W. R., Wagner, N. J., Willoughby, M. T., Stifter, C., Blair, C., & Granger, D. A. (2015). Greater fear reactivity and psychophysiological hyperactivity among infants with later conduct problems and callous-unemotional traits. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 56,147–154.Google Scholar
  52. Moore, G. A., Cohn, J. F., & Campbell, S. B. (2001). Infant affective responses to mother’s still face at 6 months differentially predict externalizing and internalizing behaviors at 18 months. Developmental Psychology, 37, 706–714. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.37.5.706.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  53. Moore, G., Hill-Soderlund, A. L., Propper, C. B., Calkins, S. D., Mills-Koonce, W. R., & Cox, M. J. (2009). Mother-infant vagal regulation in the face-to-face still-face paradigm is moderated by maternal sensitivity. Child Development, 80, 209–223. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2008.01255.x.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  54. Newman, J. P. & Lorenz, A. R. (2003). Response modulation and emotion processing: Implications for psychopathy and other dysregulatory psychopathology. In R. J. Davidson, K. R. Scherer, & H. Goldsmith (Eds.), Handbook of affective sciences (pp. 904–929). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  55. Newman, J. P., Curtin, J. J., Bertsch, J. D., & Baskin-Sommers, A. R. (2010). Attention moderates the fearlessness of psychopathic offenders. Biological Psychiatry, 67: 66–70.Google Scholar
  56. NICHD ECCRN (1999). Child care and mother-child interaction in the first three years of life. Developmental Psychology, 35, 1399–1413. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.35.6.1399.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  57. Pardini, D. A., Lochman, J. E., & Powell, N. (2007). The development of callous-unemotional traits and antisocial behavior in children: are there shared and/or unique predictors? Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 36, 319–333. doi: 10.1080/15374410701444215.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  58. Park, S. Y., Belsky, J., Putnam, S., & Crnic, K. (1997). Infant emotionality, parenting, and 3-year inhibition: exploring stability and lawful discontinuity in male sample. Developmental Psychology, 33, 218–227.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  59. Pasalich, D. S., Dadds, M. R., Hawes, D. J., & Brennan, J. (2012). Attachment and callous unemotional traits in children with early-onset conduct problems. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 53, 838–845. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2012.02544.x.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  60. Patterson, C. & Newman, J. P. (1993). Reflectivity and learning from aversive events: Toward a psychological mechanism for the syndromes of disinhibition. Psychological Review, 100, 716–736. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.100.4.716.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  61. Preacher, K. J., Curran, P. J., & Bauer, D. J. (2006). Computational tools for probing interactions in multiple linear regression, multilevel modeling, and latent curve analysis. Journal of Educational and Behavioral Statistics, 31, 437–448.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  62. Raine, A. (2002). Biosocial studies of antisocial and violent behavior in children and adults: a review. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 30, 311–326. doi: 10.1023/A:1015754122318.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  63. Raine, A., Reynolds, C., Venables, P. H., Mednick, S. A., & Farrington, D. P. (1998). Fearlessness, stimulation-seeking, and large body size at age 3 years as early predispositions to childhood aggression at age 11 years. Archives of General Psychiatry, 55, 745–751.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  64. Richell, R. A., Mitchell, D. G. V., Newman, C., Leonard, A., Baron-Cohen, S., & Blair, R. J. R. (2003). Theory of mind and psychopathy: can psychopathic individuals read the ‘language of the eyes’? Neuropsychologists, 41, 523–526.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  65. Rothbart, M. K. (1981). Measurement of temperament in infancy. Child Development, 52, 569–578.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  66. Rowe, R., Maughan, B., Moran, P., Ford, T., Briskman, J., & Goodman, R. (2010). The role of callous and unemotional traits in the diagnosis of conduct disorder. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 51, 688–695. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2009.02199.x.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  67. Scarpa, A., Haden, S. C., & Tanaka, A. (2010). Being hot-tempered: autonomic, emotional, and behavioral distinctions between childhood reactive and proactive aggression. Biological Psychology, 84, 488–496. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2009.11.006.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  68. Shaw, D. S. & Gross, H. E. (2008). What we have learned about early childhood and the development of delinquency. In A. K. Liberman (Ed.), The long view of crime: A synthesis of longitudinal research (pp. 79–127). New York: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  69. Shaw, D. S., Gilliom, M., Ingoldsby, E. M., & Nagin, D. S. (2003). Trajectories leading to school-age conduct problems. Developmental Psychology, 39, 189.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  70. Song, J., Waller, R., Hyde, L., & Olson, S. (2015). Early callous-unemotional behavior, theory-of-mind, and fearful/inhibited temperament predict externalizing problems in middle and late childhood. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. doi: 10.1007/s10802-015-0099-3.Google Scholar
  71. Spezio, M. L., Huang, P.-Y. S., Castelli, F., & Adolphs, R. (2007). Amygdala damage impairs eye contact during conversations with real people. Journal of Neuroscience, 27, 3994–3997. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3789-06.2007.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  72. Sroufe, L. A. (1996). Emotional development: The organization of emotional life in the early years. New York: Cambridge University Press. doi: 10.1017/CBO9780511527661.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  73. Sroufe, L. A., Carlson, E. A., Levy, A. K., & Egeland, B. (1999). Implications of attachment theory for developmental psychopathology. Development and Psychopathology, 11, 1–13.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  74. Stadler, C., Kroeger, A., Weyers, P., Grasmann, D., Horschinek, M., Freitag, C., et al. (2011). Cortisol reactivity in boys with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and disruptive behavior problems: the impact of callous-unemotional traits. Psychiatry Research, 187, 204–209. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2010.05.004.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  75. Stringaris, A. & Goodman, R. (2009). Longitudinal outcome of youth oppositionality: irritable, headstrong, and hurtful behaviors have distinctive predictions. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 48, 404–412. doi: 10.1097/CHI.0b013e3181984f30.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  76. Tronick, E. Z. (1989). Emotions and emotional communication in infants. American Psychologist, 44, 112–119. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.44.2.112.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  77. Tronick, E. Z., Als, H., Adamson, L., Wise, S., & Brazelton, T. B. (1978). The infant’s response to entrapment between contradictory messages in face-to-face interaction. Journal of the American Academy of Child Psychiatry, 17, 1–13.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  78. Tronick, E. Z., Bruschweiler-Stern, N., Harrison, A. M., Lyons-Ruth, K., Morgan, A. C., Nahum, J. P., et al. (1998). Dyadically expanded states of consciousness and the process of therapeutic change. Infant Mental Health Journal, 19, 290–299. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1097-0355(199823)19:3<290::AID-IMHJ4>3.0.CO;2-Q.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  79. Van Zeijl, J., Mesman, J., Van IJzendoorn, M. H., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., Juffer, F., Stolk, M. N., et al. (2006). Attachment-based intervention for enhancing sensitive discipline in mothers of 1- to 3-year-old children at risk for externalizing behavior problems: a randomized controlled trial. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 74, 994–1005. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.74.6.994.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  80. Viding, E., Blair, R. J. R., Moffitt, T. E., & Plomin, R. (2005). Evidence for substantial genetic risk for psychopathy in 7-year-olds. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 46, 592–597. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00393.x.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  81. Vitaro, F., Barker, E. D., Boivin, M., Brendgen, M., & Tremblay, R. E. (2006). Do early difficult temperament and harsh parenting differentially predict reactive and proactive aggression? Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 34, 685–695. doi: 10.1007/s10802-006-9055-6.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  82. Wagner, N. J., Mills-Koonce, W. R., Willoughby, M. T., Zvara, B., Cox, M. J., & The Family Life Project Key Investigators (2015a). Parenting and children’s representations of family predict disruptive and callous-unemotional behaviors. Developmental Psychology. doi: 10.1037/a0039353.Google Scholar
  83. Wagner, N. J., Mills-Koonce, W. R., Willoughby, M., Propper, C., Rehder, P., & Gueron-Sela, N. (2015b). Respiratory sinus arrhythmia and heart period in infancy as correlates of later oppositional defiant and callous-unemotional behaviors. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 1–9. doi: 10.1177/0165025415605391.
  84. Wagner, N. J., Propper, C., Gueron-Sela, N., & Mills-Koonce, W. R. (2015c). Dimensions of maternal parenting and infants’ autonomic functioning interactively predict early internalizing behavior problems. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. doi: 10.1007/s10802-015-0039-2.Google Scholar
  85. Waller, R., Gardner, F., Hyde, L. W., Shaw, D. S., Dishion, T. J., & Wilson, M. N. (2012). Do harsh and positive parenting predict parent reports of deceitful-callous behavior in early childhood? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 53, 946–953. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2012.02550.x.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  86. Waller, R., Gardner, F., & Hyde, L. W. (2013). What are the associations between parenting, callous–unemotional traits, and antisocial behavior in youth? A systematic review of evidence. Clinical Psychology Review, 33, 593–608. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2013.03.001.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  87. Waller, R., Gardner, F., Viding, E., Shaw, D. S., Dishion, T. J., Wilson, M. N., et al. (2014a). Bidirectional associations between parental warmth, callous unemotional behavior, and behavior problems in high-risk preschoolers. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 42, 1275–1285.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  88. Waller, R., Gardner, F., Shaw, D. S., Dishion, T. J., Wilson, M. N., & Hyde, L. W. (2014b). Callous-unemotional behavior and early-childhood onset of behavior problems: the role of parental harshness and warmth. Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology. doi: 10.1080/15374416.2014.886252.Google Scholar
  89. Waller, R., Hyde, L., Grabell, A., Alves, M., & Olson, S. (2015a). Differential associations of early callous-unemotional, oppositional, and ADHD behaviors: multiple domains within early-starting conduct problems? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12326.Google Scholar
  90. Waller, R., Shaw, D. S., Neiderhiser, J. M., Ganiban, J. M., Natsuaki, M. N., Reiss, D., et al. (2015b). Toward an understanding of the role of the environment in the development of early callous behavior. Journal of Personality. doi: 10.1111/jopy.12221.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  91. Waller, R., Wright, A. G., Shaw, D. S., Gardner, F., Dishion, T. J., & Wilson, M. N. (2015c). Factor structure and construct validity of the parent-reported inventory of callous-unemotional traits among high-risk 9-year-olds. Assessment, 22, 561–580.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  92. Waters, E. & Sroufe, L. A. (1983). Social competence as a developmental construct. Developmental Review, 3, 79–97. doi: 10.1016/0273-2297(83)90010-2.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  93. Waters, E., Wippman, J., & Sroufe, L. A. (1979). Attachment, positive affect, and competence in the peer group: two studies in construct validation. Child Development, 50, 821–829. doi: 10.2307/1128949.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  94. Weinberg, K. M. & Tronick, E. Z. (1996). Infant affective reactions to the resumption of maternal interaction after the still-face. Child Development, 67, 905–914. doi: 10.2307/1131869.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  95. Willoughby, M. T., Waschbusch, D. A., Moore, G. A., & Propper, C. B. (2011). Using the ASEBA to screen for callous-unemotional traits in early childhood: factor structure, temporal stability, and utility. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 33, 19–30. doi: 10.1007/s10862-010-9195-4.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  96. Willoughby, M. T., Mills-Koonce, W. R., Propper, C. B., & Waschbusch, D. A. (2013). Observed parenting behaviors interact with a polymorphism of the brain derived neurotrophic factor gene to predict the emergence of oppositional defiant and callous-unemotional behaviors at age 3 years. Development and Psychopathology, 25, 903–917. doi: 10.1017/S0954579413000266.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  97. Willoughby, M. T., Mills-Koonce, W. R., Gottfredson, N. C., & Wagner, N. J. (2014). Measuring callous unemotional behaviors in early childhood: factor structure and the prediction of stable aggression in middle childhood. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 36, 30–42. doi: 10.1007/s10862-013-9379-9.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media New York 2016

Authors and Affiliations

  • Nicholas J. Wagner
    • 1
  • W. Roger Mills-Koonce
    • 2
  • Cathi B. Propper
    • 3
  • Michael T. Willoughby
    • 4
  • Pete D. Rehder
    • 2
  • Ginger A. Moore
    • 5
  • Martha J. Cox
    • 6
  1. 1.Department of Psychology and NeuroscienceUniversity of North Carolina at Chapel HillChapel HillUSA
  2. 2.University of North Carolina at GreensboroGreensboroUSA
  3. 3.Center for Developmental ScienceUniversity of North Carolina at Chapel HillChapel HillUSA
  4. 4.RTI InternationalResearch Triangle ParkUSA
  5. 5.Penn State UniversityUniversity ParkUSA
  6. 6.Department of Psychology and NeuroscienceUniversity of North Carolina at Chapel HillChapel HillUSA

Personalised recommendations