Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology

, Volume 36, Issue 8, pp 1265–1278 | Cite as

Differential Genetic and Environmental Influences on Reactive and Proactive Aggression in Children

  • Laura A. Baker
  • Adrian Raine
  • Jianghong Liu
  • Kristen C. Jacobson
Article

Abstract

While significant heritability for childhood aggression has been claimed, it is not known whether there are differential genetic and environmental contributions to proactive and reactive forms of aggression in children. This study quantifies genetic and environmental contributions to these two forms of aggression in an ethnically diverse urban sample of 9–10 year old twins (N = 1219), and compares results across different informants (child self-report, mother, and teacher ratings) using the Reactive–Proactive Aggression Questionnaire (RPQ). Confirmatory factor analysis of RPQ items indicated a significant and strong fit for a two-factor proactive–reactive model which was significantly superior to a one-factor model and which replicated across gender as well as the three informant sources. Males scored significantly higher than females on both self-report reactive and proactive aggression, findings that replicated on mother and teacher versions of the RPQ. Asian–Americans scored lower than most ethnic groups on reactive aggression yet were equivalent to Caucasians on proactive aggression. African–Americans scored higher than other ethnic groups on all measures of aggression except caregiver reports. Heritable influences were found for both forms of aggression across informants, but while boys’ self-reports revealed genetic influences on proactive (50%) and reactive (38%) aggression, shared and non-shared environmental influences almost entirely accounted for girls’ self-report reactive and proactive aggression. Although genetic correlations between reactive and proactive aggression were significant across informants, there was evidence that the genetic correlation was less than unity in boys self reported aggression, indicating that genetic factors differ for proactive and reactive aggression. These findings provide the first evidence for varying genetic and environmental etiologies for reactive and proactive aggression across gender, and provide additional support for distinction between these two forms of aggression.

Keywords

Aggression Reactive Proactive Genetic Gender 

Notes

Acknowledgments

This study was supported by grants to the first author from National Institute of Mental Health (NIMH; RO1 MH58354), to the second author from NIMH (Independent Scientist Award K02 MH01114-08), to the third author from an National Institute of Nursing Research post-doctoral fellowship award (F32 NR-08661-2) and National Institute of Environmental Health Sciences (K01 ES015877-01), and to the fourth author from National Institutes of Health (K01 MH068484-01).

References

  1. Achenbach, T. M., McConaughy, S. H., & Howell, C. T. (1987). Child/adolescent behavioral and emotional problems: Implications of cross informant correlations for situational specificity. Psychological Bulletin, 101, 213–232.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Akaike, A. C. (1987). Factor analysis and AIC. Psychometrika, 52, 317–332.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Baker, L. A., Barton, M., & Raine, A. (2002). The Southern California twin register at the University of Southern California. Twin Research, 5, 456–459.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Baker, L.A., Barton, M., Lozano, D.I., Raine, A., Fowler, J. H. (2006). The Southern California twin register at the University of Southern California: II. Twin Research and Human Genetics, 9(6), 933–940.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Baker, L. A., Jacobsen, K., Raine, A., Lozano, D. I., & Bezdjian, S. (2007). Genetic and environmental bases of antisocial behavior in children: I A multi-informant twin study Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 116(2), 219–235.Google Scholar
  6. Bandura, A. (1983). Psychological mechanisms of aggression. In R. G. Green & E. I. Donnerstein (Eds.), Aggression: Theoretical and empirical reviews (vol. 1, pp. 1–40). New York: Academic.Google Scholar
  7. Bentler, P. M. (1990). Comparative fit indexes in structural models. Psychological Bulletin, 107, 238–246.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Bentler, P. M. (1995). EQS Structural equations program manual. Encino: Multivariate Software.Google Scholar
  9. Bentler, P. M., Berkane, M., & Kano, Y. (1991). Covariance structure analysis under a simple kurtosis model. In E. M. Keramidas (Ed.), Computing science and statistics (pp. 463–465). Fairfax Station: Interface Foundation of North America.Google Scholar
  10. Bentler, P. M., & Bonett, D. G. (1980). Significance tests and goodness of fit in the analysis of covariance structures. Psychological Bulletin, 88(3), 588–606.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Bentler, P. M., & Dudgeon, P. (1996). Covariance structure analysis: statistical practice, theory, and directions. Annual Review of Psychology, 47, 563–592.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Bentler, P. M., & Wu, E. J. C. (2002). EQS 6 for windows users guide. Encino: Multivariate Software.Google Scholar
  13. Berkowitz, L. (1963). Aggression: A social psychological analysis. New York: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
  14. Brendgen, M., Vitaro, F., Boivin, M., Dionne, G., & Perusse, D. (2006). Examining genetic and environmental effects on reactive versus proactive aggression. Development Psychology, 42, 1299–1312.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Bushman, B. J., & Anderson, C. A. (2001). Is it time to pull the plug on the hostile versus instrumental aggression dichotomy. Psychological Review, 108, 273–279.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Cadoret, R. J., Leve, L. D., & Devor, E. (1997). Genetics of aggressive and violent behavior. Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 20, 301–322.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Crick, N. R., & Dodge, K. A. (1996). Social information-processing mechanisms on reactive and proactive aggression. Child Development, 67, 993–1002.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Dodge, K. A. (1991). The structure and function of reactive and proactive aggression. In D. Pepler & K. Rubin (Eds.), The development and treatment for childhood aggression (pp. 201–218). Hillsdale: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  19. Dodge, K. A., & Coie, J. D. (1987). Social-information-processing factors in reactive and proactive aggression in children’s peer groups. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 53, 1146–1158.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Dodge, K. A., Lochman, J. E., Harnish, J. D., & Bates, J. E. (1997a). Reactive and proactive aggression in school children and psychiatrically impaired chronically assaultive youth. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 106, 37–51.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Dodge, K. A., Pettit, G. S., & Bates, J. E. (1997b). How the experience of early physical abuse leads children to become chronically aggressive. In D. En Cicchetti, & S. L. Toth (Eds.), Developmental perspectives on trauma: theory, research, and intervention (pp. 263–288). Rochester: University of Rochester Press.Google Scholar
  22. Edelbrock, C., Rende, R., Plomin, R., & Thompson, L. A. (1995). A twin study of competence and problem behavior in childhood and early adolescence. Journal of Child Psychology & Psychiatry & Allied Disciplines, 36, 775–785.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Grafman, J., Schwab, K., Warden, D., Pridgen, A., Brown, H. R., & Salazar, A. M. (1996). Frontal lobe injuries, violence, and aggression: a report of the Vietnam head injury study. Neurology, 46, 1231–1238.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  24. Gregg, T.R., & Siegel, A. (2001). Brain structures and neurotransmitters regulating aggression in cats: Implications for human aggression. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology & Biological Psychiatry, 25(1), 91–140.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Hudziak, J. J., van Beijsterveldt, C. E. M., Bartels, M., Rietveld, M. J. H., Rettew, D. C., & Derks, E. M. (2003). Individual differences in aggression: genetic analyses by age, gender, and informant in 3-, 7-, and 10-year-old Dutch twins. Behavior Genetics, 33, 575–589.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Kagan, J. (1992). Yesterday’s premises, tomorrow’s promises. Developmental Psychology, 28, 990–997.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Kagan, J. (1994). Galen’s prophecy: temperament in human nature. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
  28. Kano, Y., Berkane, M., & Bentler, P. M. (1990). Covariance structure analysis with heterogeneous kurtosis parameters. Biometrika, 77, 575–585.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Kempes, M., Matthys, W., Maassen, G., van Goozen, S., & van Engeland, H. (2006). A parent questionnaire for distinguishing between reactive and proactive aggression in children. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 15, 38–45.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Laird, R. D., Pettit, G. S., Dodge, K. A., & Bates, J. E. (2005). Peer relationship antecedents of delinquent behavior in late adolescence: is there evidence of demographic group differences in developmental processes? Development and Psychopathology, 17(1), 127–144.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Loehlin, J. C. (1992). Latent variable models: An introduction to factor, path, and structural analysis (p. 292). Hillsdale: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  32. McDonald, R. P. (1989). An index of goodness-of-fit based on noncentrality. Journal of Classification, 6, 97–103.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Miles, D. R., & Carey, G. (1997). Genetic and environmental architecture on human aggression. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 72, 207–217.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. McLaughlin, K. A., Hilt, L. M., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2007). Racial/ethnic differences in internalizing and externalizing symptoms in adolescents. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 35(5), 801–816.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Meloy, J.R. (1988). The psychopathic mind: Origins, dynamics, and treatment. Northvale: Jason Aronson.Google Scholar
  36. Patterson, G. R. (2002). The early development of coercive family processes. In J. B. Reid, G. R. Patterson, & J. Snyder (Eds.), Antisocial behavior in children and adolescents: A developmental analysis and model for intervention (pp. 25–44). Washington, D.C.: American Psychological Association.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Plomin, R., Nitz, K., & Rowe, D. C. (1990). Behavioral genetics and aggressive behavior in childhood. In M. Lewis & S. M. Miller (Eds.), Handbook of developmental psychopathology: Perspectives in developmental psychology (pp. 119–133). New York: Plenum.Google Scholar
  38. Poulin, F., & Boivin, M. (2000). Reactive and proactive aggression: Evidence of a two-factor model. Psychological Assessment, 12, 115–122.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Poulin, F., Cillessen, A. H. N., Hubbard, J. A., Coie, J. D., Dodge, K. A., & Schwartz, D. (1997). Children’s friends and behavioral similarity in two social contexts. Social development, 6, 224–236.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Price, J. M., & Dodge, K. A. (1989). Reactive and proactive aggression in childhood: relations to peer status and social context dimensions. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 17, 455–471.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Raine, A., Dodge, K., Loeber, R., Gatzke-Kopp, L., Lynam, D., & Reynolds, C. (2006). The Reactive-Proactive Aggression (RPQ) questionnaire: differential correlates of reactive and proactive aggression in adolescent boys. Aggressive Behavior, 32, 159–171.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Raine, A., Meloy, J. R., Bihrle, S., Stoddard, J., LaCasse, L., & Buchsbaum, M. S. (1998). Reduced prefrontal and increased subcortical brain functioning assessed using positron emission tomography in predatory and affective murderers. Behavioral Sciences & the Law, 16, 319–332.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Rhee, S. H., & Waldman, I. D. (2002). Genetic and environmental influences on antisocial behavior: A meta-analysis of twin and adoption studies. Psychological Bulletin, 128, 490–529.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Satorra, P. M., & Bentler, A. (2001). A scaled difference chi-square test statistic for moment structure analysis. Psychometrika, 66, 507–514.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Simonoff, E., Pickles, A., Meyer, J., Silberg, J., & Maes, H. (1998). Genetic and environmental influences on subtypes of conduct disorder behavior in boys. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 26, 495–509.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  46. Thompson, P. M., Cannon, T. D., Narr, K. L., van Erp, T., Poutanen, V. P., & Huttunen, M. (2001). Genetic influences on brain structure. Nature Neuroscience, 4, 1253–1258.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Tomada, G., & Schneider, B. H. (1997). Relational aggression, gender, and peer acceptance: Invariance across culture, stability over time, and concordance among informants. Developmental Psychology, 33, 601–609.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2008

Authors and Affiliations

  • Laura A. Baker
    • 1
  • Adrian Raine
    • 2
  • Jianghong Liu
    • 3
  • Kristen C. Jacobson
    • 4
  1. 1.Department of PsychologyUniversity of Southern CaliforniaLos AngelesUSA
  2. 2.Departments of Criminology, Psychiatry, and PsychologyUniversity of PennsylvaniaPhiladelphiaUSA
  3. 3.School of Nursing and School of MedicineUniversity of PennsylvaniaPhiladelphiaUSA
  4. 4.Department of PsychiatryUniversity of ChicagoChicagoUSA

Personalised recommendations