Advertisement

Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology

, Volume 36, Issue 2, pp 135–150 | Cite as

Children with Autism and Their Friends: A Multidimensional Study of Friendship in High-Functioning Autism Spectrum Disorder

  • Nirit BaumingerEmail author
  • Marjorie Solomon
  • Anat Aviezer
  • Kelly Heung
  • Lilach Gazit
  • John Brown
  • Sally J. Rogers
Article

Abstract

This study of Israeli and American preadolescent children examined characteristics of friendship in 44 children with high-functioning autism spectrum disorder (HFASD) compared to 38 typically developing children (TYP), as they interacted with a close friend Participants were 8–12 years of age (HFASD: Israel, n = 24; USA, n = 20; TYP: Israel, n = 23; USA, n = 15), and were matched on SES, receptive language vocabulary, child age, and gender (each study group included one girl). Multidimensional assessments included: individual behaviors of target children and observed child–friend interactions during construction and drawing scenarios; target child’s and friend’s self-perceived mutual friendship qualities; and mother-reported characteristics (friendship’s duration/frequency; friend’s age/gender/disability status). Overall, children with HFASD displayed a number of differences on individual and dyadic friendship measures. Both age and verbal abilities affected friendship behaviors. Children with HFASD and their friends perceived friendship qualities similarly, suggesting that preadolescents with HFASD have capacities for interpersonal awareness. Between-group similarities also emerged on several complex social behaviors, suggesting that friendship follows a developmental trajectory in autism and may enhance social interaction skills in autism.

Keywords

Children with high-functioning autism spectrum disorder (HFASD) Asperger syndrome Friendship Multidimensional assessment 

Notes

Acknowledgements

This research was supported by a grant from the United States–Israel Binational Science Foundation (BSF) to Drs. Rogers and Bauminger. Special thanks are extended to the children who took part in this study. The authors would like to express their appreciation to Dee B. Ankonina for her editorial contribution and to Dov Har-Even for his statistical assistance.

References

  1. American Psychiatric Association (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.Google Scholar
  2. American Psychiatric Association (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th-TR ed.). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.Google Scholar
  3. Asher, S. R., Parker, J. G., & Walker, D. L. (1996). Distinguishing friendship from acceptance: Implications for intervention and assessment. In W. M. Bukowski, A. F. Newcomb, & W. W. Hartup (Eds.), The company they keep: Friendships in childhood and adolescence (pp. 366–406). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  4. Attwood, T. (1998). Asperger’s syndrome: A guide for parents and professionals. London: Jessica Kingsley.Google Scholar
  5. Bagwell, C. L., Newcomb, A. F., & Bukowski, W. M. (1998). Preadolescent friendship and peer rejection as predictors of adult adjustment. Child Development, 69, 140–153.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Bauminger, N., & Kasari, C. (2000). Loneliness and friendship in high-functioning children with autism. Child Development, 71, 447–456.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Bauminger, N., & Shulman, C. (2003). The development and maintenance of friendship in high-functioning children with autism: Maternal perception. Autism, 7, 81–97.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Bauminger, N., Rogers, S. J., Aviezer, A., & Solomon, M. (2005). The friendship observation scale (FOS). Unpublished manual, Bar Ilan University, Israel and University of California, Davis, CA.Google Scholar
  9. Brachfield-Child, S., & Schiavo, R. S. (1990). Interactions of preschool and kindergarten friends and acquaintances. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 151, 45–57.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Brody, G. H., Stoneman, Z., & McKinnon, C. E. (1982). Role asymmetries in interactions among school-age children, their younger siblings and their friends. Child Development, 53, 1364–1370.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Buhrmester, D. (1996). Need fulfillment, interpersonal competence, and the developmental context of early adolescent friendship. In W. M. Bukowski, A. F., Newcomb, & W. W. Hartup (Eds.), The company they keep: Friendship in childhood and adolescence (pp.158–185). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  12. Bukowski, W. M., Boivin, M., & Hoza, B. (1994). Measuring friendship quality during pre- and early adolescence: The development and psychometric properties of the friendship qualities scale. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 11, 471–484.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Burgess, K. B., Wojslawowicz, J. C., Rubin, K. H., Rose-Krasnor, L., & Booth-LaForce, C. (2006). Social information processing and coping strategies of shy/withdrawn and aggressive children: Does friendship matter? Child Development, 77, 371–383.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Capps, L., Sigman, M., & Mundy, P. (1994). Attachment security in children with autism. Development and Psychopathology, 6, 249–261.Google Scholar
  15. Carrington, S., & Graham, L. (2001). Perceptions of school by two teenage boys with Asperger syndrome and their mothers: A qualitative study. Autism, 5, 37–48.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Carrington, S., Templeton, E., & Papinczak, T. (2003). Adolescents with Asperger syndrome and perceptions of friendship. Focus on Autism and Other Developmental Disabilities, 18, 211–218.Google Scholar
  17. Church, C., Alinsanski, S., & Amanullah, S. (2000). The social, behavioural, and academic experiences of children with Asperger syndrome. Focus on Autism and Other Developmental Disabilities, 15, 12–20.Google Scholar
  18. Dunn, J. (1993). Young children’s close relationships: Beyond attachment. London: Sage.Google Scholar
  19. Dunn, L. M., & Dunn, L. M. (1997). Examiner’s manual for the Peabody picture vocabulary test third edition. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.Google Scholar
  20. Field, T., Greenwald, P., Marrow, C., Healy, B., Foster, T., Guthertz, M., et al. (1992). Behavior state matching during interactions of preadolescent friends versus acquaintances. Developmental Psychology, 28, 242–250.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Frith, U. (2004). Emanuel Miller lecture: Confusion and controversies about Asperger syndrome. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45, 672–686.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Green, J., Gilchrist, A., Burton, D., & Cox, A. (2000). Social and psychiatric functioning in adolescents with Asperger syndrome compared with conduct disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 30, 279–293.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Hermelin, B., & O’Connor, N. (1985). Logico-affective states and nonverbal language. In E. Schopler, & G. B. Mesibov (Eds.), Communication problems in autism (pp. 283–309). New York: Plenum.Google Scholar
  24. Hinde, R. (1979). Towards understanding relationships. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  25. Hobson, P. (2005). Autism and emotion. In F. R. Volkmar, R. Paul, A. Klin, & D. Cohen (Eds.), Handbook of autism and pervasive developmental disorders (pp. 406–422). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.Google Scholar
  26. Hobson, P., Chidambi, G., Lee, A., & Meyer, J. (2006). Foundations of self-awareness: An exploration through autism. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 71(1, Serial No. 284).Google Scholar
  27. Howes, C. (1996). The earliest friendships. In W. M. Bukowski, A. F., Newcomb, & W. W. Hartup (Eds.), The company they keep: Friendship in childhood and adolescence (pp. 66–86). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  28. Kanner, L. (1943). Autistic disturbances of affective contact. Nervous Child, 2, 217–250.Google Scholar
  29. Kasari, C., Chamberlain, B., & Bauminger, N. (2001). Social emotions and social relationships in autism: Can children with autism compensate? In J. Burack, T. Charman, N. Yirmiya, & P. Zelazo (Eds.), Perspectives on development in autism (pp. 309–323). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  30. Koning, C., & Magill-Evans, J. (2001). Social and language skills in adolescent boys with Asperger syndrome. Autism, 5, 23–36.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Landa, R. (2000). Social language use in Asperger syndrome and in high-functioning autism. In A. Klin, F. R. Volkmar, & S. S. Sparrow (Eds.), Asperger syndrome (pp. 121–155). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  32. Lemerise, E. A., & Arsenio, W. F. (2000). An integral model of emotion processes and cognition in social information processing. Child Development, 71, 107–118.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Liss, M., Fein, D., Allen, D., Dunn, M., Feinstein, C., Norris, R., et al. (2001). Executive functioning in high-functioning children with autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 42, 261–270.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Lord, C. (1984). The development of peer relations in children with autism. In F. J. Morrison, C. Lord, & D. P. Keating (Eds.), Advances in applied developmental psychology (pp. 165–229). New York: Academic.Google Scholar
  35. Lord, C., Rutter, M., DiLavore, P. C., & Risi, S. (1999). The autism diagnostic observation schedule: The manual. Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services.Google Scholar
  36. Lord, C., Rutter, M., & LeCouteur, A. (1994). Autism diagnostic interview-revised: A revised version of a diagnostic interview for caregivers of individuals with possible pervasive developmental disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 19, 185–212.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Macintosh, K. E., & Dissanayake, C. (2004). Annotation: The similarities and differences between autistic disorder and Asperger’s disorder: A review of empirical evidence. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45, 421–434.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Newcomb, A. F., & Brady, J. E. (1982). Mutuality in boys’ friendship relations. Child Development, 53, 392–395.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Orsmond, G., Krauss, M., & Seltzer, M. (2004). Peer relationships and social and recreational activities among adolescents and adults with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disabilities, 34, 245–256.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Park, K. A., & Waters, E. (1989). Security of attachment and preschool friendships. Child Development, 60, 1076–1081.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Parker, J. G., & Gottman, J. M. (1989). Social and emotional development in a relational context: Friendship interaction from early childhood to adolescence. In T. Brendt, & G. Ladd (Eds.), Peer relationships in child development (pp. 95–131). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  42. Parker, J. G., Rubin, K. H., Price, J. M., & DeRosier, M. E. (1995). Peer relationships, child development, and adjustment: A developmental psychopathology perspective. In D. Cicchetti, & D. J. Cohen (Eds.), Developmental psychopathology (pp. 96–161). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  43. Rogers, S. R., & Pennington, B. F. (1991). A theoretical approach to the deficits in infantile autism. Development and Psychopathology, 3, 137–162.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Sameroff, A. J. (1983). Developmental systems: Contexts and evolution. In P. H. Mussen & W. Kessen (Eds.), Handbook of child psychiatry: Vol. 1. History, theories and methods (pp. 237–294). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  45. Sattler, J. M. (1988). Assessment of children (3rd ed.). San Diego: Author.Google Scholar
  46. Shany, M., Lachman, D., Shalem, Z., Bahat, A., & Zeiger, T. (2003). “Ma’akav:” Current mapping of reading and writing based on Israeli norms. Tel Aviv: Yesod. (Hebrew).Google Scholar
  47. Sigman, M., & Ruskin, E. (1999). Continuity and change in the social competence of children with autism, Down Syndrome, and developmental delays. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 64(1, Serial No. 256).Google Scholar
  48. Siperstein, G. N., Leffert, J. S., & Wenz-Gross, M. (1997). The quality of friendships between children with and without learning problems. American Journal of Mental Retardation, 102, 111–125.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Stern, D. (1985). The interpersonal world of the infant: View from psychoanalysis and developmental psychology. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
  50. Sullivan, H. S. (1953). The interpersonal theory of psychiatry. New York: Horton.Google Scholar
  51. Tager-Flusberg, H. (2001). A reexamination of the theory of mind hypothesis of autism. In J. Burack, T. Charman, N. Yirmiya, & P. Zelazo (Eds.), Development and autism: Perspectives from theory and research (pp. 173–193). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  52. Volkmar, F. R., Lord, C., Bailey, A. Schultz, R. T., & Klin, A. (2004). Autism and pervasive developmental disorder. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45, 135–170.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Waters, E., & Deane, K. E. (1985). Defining and assessing individual differences in attachment relationships: Q—methodology and the organization of behavior in infancy and early childhood. In I. Bretherton, & E. Waters (Eds.), Growing points in attachment theory and research, Monographs of the society for research in child development (pp. 41–65). 50(1–2, Serial No. 209).Google Scholar
  54. Wilkinson, G. S. (1993). WRAT-3: Wide range achievement test administration manual. Wilmington, DE: Wide Range.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2007

Authors and Affiliations

  • Nirit Bauminger
    • 1
    Email author
  • Marjorie Solomon
    • 2
  • Anat Aviezer
    • 1
  • Kelly Heung
    • 2
  • Lilach Gazit
    • 1
  • John Brown
    • 2
  • Sally J. Rogers
    • 2
  1. 1.School of EducationBar Ilan UniversityRamat GanIsrael
  2. 2.Mind InstituteUniversity of CaliforniaDavisUSA

Personalised recommendations