Advertisement

Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology

, Volume 34, Issue 6, pp 850–862 | Cite as

Online Story Comprehension among Children with ADHD: Which Core Deficits are Involved?

  • Kate Flory
  • Richard Milich
  • Elizabeth P. Lorch
  • Angela N. Hayden
  • Chandra Strange
  • Richard Welsh
Original Paper

Abstract

Children with ADHD have difficulty understanding causal connections and goal plans within stories. This study examined mediators of group differences in story narrations between children ages 7–9 with and without ADHD, including as potential mediators both the core deficits of ADHD (i.e., inattention, disinhibition, planning/working memory) as well measures of phonological processing and verbal skills. Forty-nine children with ADHD and 67 non-referred children narrated a wordless book and completed tasks assessing the core deficits of ADHD, phonological processing, and verbal skills. Results revealed that, although no shorter than those of non-referred children, the narratives of children with ADHD contained fewer elements relating to the story’s causal structure and goal plan. Deficits in sustained attention accounted for the most variance in these differences. Results have implications for understanding and ameliorating the academic problems experienced by children with ADHD.

Keywords

Story comprehension ADHD Core deficits Inattention 

Notes

Acknowledgements

This research was funded by grant #MH47386 from the National Institute of Mental Health. The authors wish to acknowledge the assistance of Mark Fillmore and Kristen Berthiaume in the completion of this study.

References

  1. Achenbach, T. M. (1991). Child Behavior Checklist for ages 4–18. Burlington: University of Vermont Department of Psychiatry.Google Scholar
  2. American Psychiatric Association. (1968). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (2nd ed.). Washington, DC: Author.Google Scholar
  3. American Psychiatric Association. (1980). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (3rd ed.). Washington, DC: Author.Google Scholar
  4. American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: Author.Google Scholar
  5. Andreou, G., Agapitou, P., & Karapetsas, A. (2005). Verbal skills in children with ADHD. European Journal of Special Needs Education, 20, 231–238.Google Scholar
  6. Barkley, R. A. (1997). ADHD and the nature of self-control. New York: Guilford.Google Scholar
  7. Barkley, R. A. (1998). Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: A handbook for diagnosis and treatment (2nd ed.). New York: The Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  8. Bruck, M. (1988). The word recognition and spelling of dyslexic children. Reading Research Quarterly, 23, 51–69.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Castellanos, F. X., & Tannock, R. (2002). Neuroscience of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Nature Reviews: Neuroscience, 3, 617–628.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  10. Culbertson, W. C., & Zillmer, E. A. (2000). Tower of LondonDX. North Tonawanda, NY: Multi-Health Systems Inc.Google Scholar
  11. Douglas, V. I. (1983). Attention and cognitive problems. In M. Rutter (Ed.), Developmental neuropsychiatry (pp. 280–329). New York: Guilford.Google Scholar
  12. Faraone, S. V., Biederman, J., Lehman, B. K., Spencer, T., Norman, D., Seidman, L. J., et al. (1993). Intellectual performance and school failure in children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and their siblings. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 102, 616–623.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Giancola, P. (2000). Executive functioning: A conceptual framework for alcohol related aggression. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology, 8, 576–597.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Lorch, E. P., Berthiaume, K. S., Milich, R., & Van Den Broek, P. (in press). Story comprehension impairments in children with ADHD. In K. Cain and J. V. Oakhill (Eds.), Cognitive bases of children’s language comprehension difficulties. New York: Guilford Publications, Inc.Google Scholar
  15. Lorch, E. P., Sanchez, R. P., Van Den Broek, P., Milich, R., Murphy, E. L., Lorch, R. Jr., et al. (1999). The relation of story structure properties to recall of television stories in young children with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder and nonreferred peers. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 27, 293–309.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. MacKinnon, D. P., Lockwood, C. M., Hoffman, J. M., West, S. G., & Sheets, V. (2002). A comparison of methods to test mediation and other intervening variables. Psychological Methods, 7, 83–104.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Mandler, J. M., & Johnson, N. S. (1977). Remembrance of things parsed: story structure and recall. Cognitive Psychology, 9, 111–151.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Mayer, M. (1967). A boy, a dog, and a frog. New York: Dial.Google Scholar
  19. Mayer, M. (1969). Frog, where are you? New York: Dial.Google Scholar
  20. Milch-Reich, S., Campbell, S. B., Pelham W. E., Jr., Connelly, L. M., & Geva, D. (1999). Developmental and individual differences in children’s online representations of dynamic social events. Child Development, 70, 413–431.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Milich, R., Balentine, A., & Lynam, D. R. (2001). ADHD combined type and ADHD predominantly inattentive type are distinct and unrelated disorders. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 8(4), 463–488.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Milich, R., Lorch, E. P., & Berthiaume, K. (2005). Story comprehension in children with ADHD: Research findings and treatment implications. In M. P. Larimer (Ed.), Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder Research (pp. 111–137). Hauppauge, NY: Nova Sciences.Google Scholar
  23. Nezworski, T., Stein, N. L., & Trabasso, T. (1982). Story structure versus content in children’s recall. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 21, 196–206.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Nigg, J. T. (2001). Is ADHD a disinhibitory disorder? Psychological Bulletin, 127, 571–598.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Nigg, J. T., Blaskey, L., Huang-Pollack, C., & Rappley, M. D. (2002). Neuropsychological executive functions and DSM-IV subtypes. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 41, 59–66.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. O’Brien, E. J., & Myers, J. L. (1987). The role of causal connections in the retrieval of text. Memory and Cognition, 15, 419–427.Google Scholar
  27. Pennington, B. F., Grossier, D., & Welsh, M. C. (1993). Contrasting cognitive deficits in attention deficit disorder versus reading disability. Developmental Psychology, 29, 511–523.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Purvis, K. L., & Tannock, R. (2000). Phonological processing, not inhibitory control, differentiates ADHD and reading disability. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 39, 485–494.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Quay, H. C. (1988). The behavioral reward and inhibition systems in childhood behavior disorders. In L. M. Bloomingdale (Ed.), Attention deficit disorder III: New research in treatment, psychopharmacology, and attention (pp. 176–186). New York: Pergamon.Google Scholar
  30. Renz, K., Lorch, E. P., Milich, R., Lemberger, C., Bodner, A., & Welsh, R. (2003). Online story representation in boys with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 31, 93–104.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Rosenthal, R., & Rosnow, R. L. (1991). Essentials of behavioral research: Methods and data analysis (2nd ed.). New York, McGraw Hill.Google Scholar
  32. Schachar, R., & Logan, G. D. (1990). Impulsivity and inhibitory control in normal development and childhood psychopathology. Developmental Psychology, 26, 710–720.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Semrud-Clikeman, M., Biederman, J., Sprich-Buckminster, S., Lehman, B. K., Faraone, S., & Norman, D. (1992). Comorbidity between ADDH and learning disability: A review and report in a clinically referred sample. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 31, 439–448.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Trabasso, T., & Sperry, L. L. (1985). Causal relatedness and importance of story events. Journal of Memory and Learning, 24, 595–611.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Trabasso, T., Stein, N. L., Rodkin, P. C., Munger, M. P., & Baughn, C. R. (1992). Knowledge of goals and plans in the online narration of events. Cognitive Development, 7, 133–170.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Trabasso, T., & Van Den Broek, P. (1985). Causal thinking and the representation of narrative events. Journal of Memory and Language, 24, 612–630.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Van Den Broek, P. (1989). Causal reasoning and inference- making in judging the importance of story statements. Child Development, 60, 286–297.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Wagner, R. K., & Torgesen, J. K. (1987). The nature of phonological processing and its causal role in the acquisition of reading skills. Psychological Bulletin, 101, 192–212.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Wechsler, D. (1991). WISC-III manual. San Antonio, TX: Harcourt Brace & Company.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2006

Authors and Affiliations

  • Kate Flory
    • 1
  • Richard Milich
    • 2
  • Elizabeth P. Lorch
    • 2
  • Angela N. Hayden
    • 2
  • Chandra Strange
    • 2
  • Richard Welsh
    • 3
  1. 1.Department of PsychologyUniversity of South Carolina, Barnwell CollegeColumbiaUSA
  2. 2.Department of PsychologyUniversity of KentuckyLexingtonUSA
  3. 3.Department of PsychiatryUniversity of KentuckyLexingtonUSA

Personalised recommendations