Advertisement

Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology

, Volume 34, Issue 5, pp 672–680 | Cite as

Attention Processes in Children with Movement Difficulties, Reading Difficulties or Both

  • S. A. Cruddace
  • P. M. RiddellEmail author
OriginalPaper

Abstract

Reading difficulties (RD) and movement difficulties (MD) co-occur more often in clinical populations than expected for independent disorders. In this study, we investigated the pattern of association between attentional processes, RD and MD in a population of 9 year old school children. Children were screened to identify index groups with RD, MD or both, plus a control group. These groups were then tested on a battery of cognitive attention assessments (TEA-Ch). Results confirmed that the occurrence of RD and MD was greater than would be predicted for independent disorders. Additionally, children with MD, whether or not combined with RD, had poor performance on all attention measures when compared with typically developing children. Children with RD only, were no poorer on measures of attention than typical children. The results are discussed with respect to approaches proposed to account for the co-occurrence of disorders.

Keywords

Reading disorder Movement disorder Attention deficit Attention Children 

Notes

Acknowledgements

We would like to thank the children, teachers and parents who participated in this research. We would also like to thank the anonymous referees who commented on a previous version of this manuscript. This research was made possible by a RETF studentship from the University of Reading to the first author.

References

  1. Allen, G., Buxton, R. B., Wong, E. C., & Courchesne, E. (1997). Attentional activation of the cerebellum independent of motor involvement. Science, 275(5308), 1940–1943.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC.Google Scholar
  3. Barkley, R. A. (1997). Behavioural inhibition, sustained attention, and executive functions: Constructing a unifying theory of ADHD. Psychological Bulletin, 121(1), 65–94.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Burton, M. W. (2001). The role of the inferior frontal cortex in phonological processing. Cognitive Science, 25(5), 695–709.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Caron, C., & Rutter, M. (1991). Comorbidity in child psychology: Concepts, issues and research strategies. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 32(7), 1063–1080.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  6. Casco, C., Tressoldi, P. E., & Dellantonio, A. (1998). Visual selective attention and reading efficiency are related in children. Cortex, 34, 531–546.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  7. Cornelissen, P., Richardson, A., Mason, A., Fowler, M. S., & Stein, J. (1995). Contrast sensitivity and coherent motion detection measured at phototopic luminance levels in dyslexics and controls. Vision Research, 35(10), 1483–1494.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Diamond, A. (2000). Close interrelation of motor development and cognitive development of the cerebellum and prefrontal cortex. Child Development, 7(1), 44–56.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Elliott, C. D. (1996). British ability scales (2nd ed.). Windsor, UK: NFER-NELSON.Google Scholar
  10. Geuze, R. H., & Kalverboer, A. F. (1994). Tapping a rhythm: A problem of timing for children who are clumsy and dyslexic? Adapted Physical Activity Quarterly, 11, 203–213.Google Scholar
  11. Goodman, R. (1991) Strengths and difficulties questionnaire. London: Institute of Psychiatry.Google Scholar
  12. Gottardo, A., Stanovich, K., & Siegel, L. (1996). The relationships between phonological sensitivity, syntactic processing, and verbal working memory in the reading performance of third grade children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 63(3), 563–582.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Henderson, S. E., & Sugden, D. A. (1992). Movement assessment battery for children. Kent, UK: The Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
  14. Joubert, S., Beauregard, M., Walter, N., Bourgouin, P., Leroux, J. M., Karama, S., et al. (2004). Neural correlates of lexical and sublexical processes in reading. Brain and Language, 89(1), 9–20.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Kadesjo, B., & Gillberg, C. (1998). Attention deficits and clumsiness in 7-year old Swedish children. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 40, 796–804.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Kaplan, B. J., Wilson, B. N., Dewey, D., & Crawford, S. G. (1998). DCD may not be a discrete disorder. Human Movement Science, 17, 471–490.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Klorman, R., Hazel-Fernandez, L. A., Shaywitz, S. E., Fletcher, J. M., Marchione, K. E., Holahan, et al. (1999). Executive functioning deficits in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder are independent of oppositional defiant or reading disorder. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 38, 1148–1155.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Lazar, J. W., & Frank, Y. (1998). Frontal systems dysfunction in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and learning disabilities. Journal of Neuropsychiatry, 10(2), 160–167.Google Scholar
  19. Manly, T., Robertson, I. H., Anderson, V., & Nimmo-Smith, I. (1999). The test of everyday attention for children. Bury St. Edmunds, UK: The Thames Valley Test Company Ltd.Google Scholar
  20. Mirsky, A. F., Anthony, B. J., Duncan, C. C., Ahearn, M. B., & Kellam, S. G. (1991). Analysis of the elements of attention: A neuropsychological approach. Neuropsychology Review, 2, 109–145.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Nicolson, R. I., & Fawcett, A. J. (1990). Automaticity: a new framework for dyslexia research? Cognition, 35(2) 159–182.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Nicolson, R. I., Fawcett, A. J., Berry, E. L., Jenkins, I. H., Dean, P., & Brooks, D. J. (1999). Association of abnormal cerebellar activation with motor learning difficulties in dyslexic adults. Lancet, 353(9165), 1662–1667.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Piek, J. P., Pitcher, T. M., & Hay, D. A. (1999). Motor coordination and kinaesthetics in boys with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 41, 159–165.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Posner, M., & Peterson, S. I. (1990). The attention system of the human brain. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 13, 25–42.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Pugh, K. R., Mencl, W. E., Jenner, A. R., Katz, L., Frost, S. J., Lee, J. R., et al. (2000). Functional neuroimaging studies of reading and reading disability (developmental dyslexia). Mental Retardation and Developmental Disabilities Research Review.Google Scholar
  26. Pugh, K. R., Mencl, L. E., Jenner, A. R., Katz, L., Frost, S. J., Lee, J. R., et al. (2001). Neurobiological studies of reading and reading disability. Journal of Communication Disorders, 34(6), 479–492.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Robertson, I. H., and Manly, T. (1999). Sustained attention deficits in time and space. In G. W. Humphreys, J. Duncan, & A. Treisman (Eds.), Attention, space and action (pp. 297–310). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  28. Rowe, J., Friston, K., Frackowiak, R., & Passingham, R. (2002). Attention to action: Specific modulations of corticocortical interactions in humans. Neuroimage, 17(2), 988–998.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Sergeant, J. A. (2005). Modeling attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A critical appraisal of the cognitive-energetic model. Biological Psychiatry, 57, 1248–1255.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Snowling, M. J., Stothard, S. E., & MacLean, J. M. (1996). Graded non-word reading test. Windsor, UK: NFER-NELSON.Google Scholar
  31. Stein, J. (1993). Visuospatial perception in disabled readers. In D. Willows, R. S. Kruk, & E. Corcos (Eds.), Visual processes in reading and reading disabilities. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  32. Stein, J., & Walsh, V. (1997). To see but not to read: The magnocellular theory of dyslexia. Trends in Neurosciences, 20(4), 147–152.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Talcott, J., Hansen, P., Elikem, L., & Stein, J. (2000). Visual motion sensitivity in dyslexia: Evidence for temporal and energy integration deficits. Neuropsychologia, 38, 935–943.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Thompson, M. E. (1982). The assessment of children with specific reading difficulties (dyslexia) using the British Ability Scales. British Journal of Psychology, 73, 461–478.Google Scholar
  35. Willcutt, E. G., Pennington, B. F., Olson, R. K., Chhabildas, N., & Hulslander, J. (2005). Neuropsychological analyses of comorbidity between Reading Disability and Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder: In search of the common deficit. Developmental Neuropsychology, 27(1), 35–78.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Wimmer, H., Mayringer, H., & Raberger, T. (1999). Reading and dual-task balancing: Evidence against the automatization deficit explanation of developmental dyslexia. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 32(5), 473–478.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  37. Wolf, M., Bowers, P. G., & Biddle, K. (2000). Naming-speed processes, timing, and reading: A conceptual review. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 33(4), 387–407.PubMedGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2006

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.School of Psychology and Clinical Language SciencesUniversity of ReadingReadingUK

Personalised recommendations