Advertisement

Identifying divergent design thinking through the observable behavior of service design novices

  • Ying HuEmail author
  • Xing Du
  • Nick Bryan-Kinns
  • Yinman Guo
Article

Abstract

Design thinking holds the key to innovation processes, but is often difficult to detect because of its implicit nature. We undertook a study of novice designers engaged in team-based design exercises in order to explore the correlation between design thinking and designers’ physical (observable) behavior and to identify new, objective, design thinking identification methods. Our study addresses the topic by using data collection method of “think aloud” and data analysis method of “protocol analysis” along with the unconstrained concept generation environment. Collected data from the participants without service design experience were analyzed by open and selective coding. Through the research, we found correlations between physical activity and divergent thinking, and also identified physical behaviors that predict a designer’s transition to divergent thinking. We conclude that there are significant relations between designers’ design thinking and the behavioral features of their body and face. This approach opens possible new ways to undertake design process research and also design capability evaluation.

Keywords

Design thinking Behavior recognition Novices Service design Concept generation 

References

  1. Adams, R., & Atman, C. (1999). Cognitive processes in iterative design behavior. In Frontiers in Education Conference, 1999. FIE ‘99. 29th Annual, 1, 11A6/13-11A6/18.Google Scholar
  2. Alexiou, K., Zamenopoulos, T., & Gilbert, S. (2011). Imaging the designing brain: a neurocognitive exploration of design thinking. Design Computing & Cognition'10, 489–504.Google Scholar
  3. Ball, L. J., & Ormerod, T. C. (1995). Structured and opportunistic processing in design: A critical discussion. International Journal of Human-Computer Studies, 43(1), 131–151.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Beaty, R. E., Benedek, M., Silvia, P. J., & Schacter, D. L. (2016). Creative cognition and brain network dynamics. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 20(2), 87–95.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Behoora, I., & Tucker, C. S. (2015). Machine learning classification of design team members’ body language patterns for real time emotional state detection. Design Studies, 39, 100–127.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Buchanan, R. (1992). Wicked problems in design thinking. Design Issues, 8(2), 5–21.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Cash, P., Stanković, T., & Štorga, M. (2014). Using visual information analysis to explore complex patterns in the activity of designers. Design Studies, 35(1), 1–28.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Christiaans, H., & Dorst, K. (1992). Cognitive models in industrial design engineering: A protocol study. Design Theory and Methodology, 42(2), 131–140.Google Scholar
  9. Convertino, G., Ganoe, C. H., Schafer, W. A., Yost, B., & Carroll, J. M. (2005). A multiple view approach to support common ground in distributed and synchronous geo-collaboration. In Coordinated and multiple views in exploratory visualization (pp. 121–132).Google Scholar
  10. Cross, N. (2008). Engineering design methods. Hoboken: Wiley.Google Scholar
  11. Cross, N., Christiaans, H., & Dorst, K. (1996). Analysing design activity. Materials and Design, 16(2), 122–123.Google Scholar
  12. Dorst, C. H. (1997). Describing design: a comparison of paradigms. Industrial Design Engineering, 25–81.Google Scholar
  13. Dorst, K., & Cross, N. (2001). Creativity in the design process: Co-evolution of problem–solution. Design Studies, 22(5), 425–437.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Dreyfus, H. L. (2003). The Spinoza Lectures. Amsterdam: University of Amsterdam.Google Scholar
  15. Dubberly, H. (2012). How do you design? A compendium of models (Vol. 10(8)). San Francisco: Dubberly Design Office.Google Scholar
  16. Field, A. (2013). Discovering statistics using IBM SPSS statistics (pp. 397–423). Thousand Oaks: Sage Publications Ltd.Google Scholar
  17. Fisher, J. A., Jacob, D. J., Purdy, M. T., Kopacz, M., Sager, P. L., Carouge, C., et al. (2010). Source attribution and interannual variability of arctic pollution in spring constrained by aircraft (arctas, arcpac) and satellite (airs) observations of carbon monoxide. Atmospheric Chemistry and Physics, 10(3), 977–996.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Gero, J. S., & Neill, T. M. (1998). An approach to the analysis of design protocols. Design Studies, 19(1), 21–61.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Gunes, H., & Piccardi, M. (2007). Bi-modal emotion recognition from expressive face and body gestures. Journal of Network & Computer Applications, 30(4), 1334–1345.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Klein, G. (1998). Sources of Power: How people make Decisions (pp. 1–323). Cambridge: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  21. Knight, H., & Simmons, R. (2013). Estimating human interest and attention via gaze analysis. In IEEE international conference on robotics and automation (pp. 4350–4355).Google Scholar
  22. Lawson, B. (2004). Schemata, gambits and precedent: some factors in design expertise. Design Studies, 25(5), 443–457.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Lawson, B., & Dorst, K. (2009). Design expertise. Design expertise (pp. 98–99). Amsterdam: Elsevier.Google Scholar
  24. Louwerse, M. M., Dale, R., Bard, E. G., & Jeuniaux, P. (2012). Behavior matching in multimodal communication is synchronized. Cognitive Science, 36(8), 1404–1426.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Mahmoud, M., & Robinson, P. (2011). Interpreting hand-over-face gestures. International Conference on Affective Computing and Intelligent Interaction, 6975, 248–255.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Manhas, C., & Bakhshi, A. (2011). Relating occupational self efficacy to team effectiveness. European Journal of Business & Management, 3(2), 165–172.Google Scholar
  27. Mari, D. D., & Kotz, S. (2001). Correlation and Dependence. Correlation and dependence: Imperial College Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Park, S., Scherer, S., Gratch, J., Carnevale, P., & Morency, L. P. (2013). Mutual behaviors during dyadic negotiation: Automatic prediction of respondent reactions. In T. Pun, C. Pelachaud, & N. Sebe (Eds.), 2013 Humaine Association Conference on Affective Computing and Intelligent Interaction (ACII) (pp. 423–428). Los Alomitos, CA: IEEE.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Pedgley, O. (2007). Capturing and analysing own design activity. Design Studies, 28(5), 463–483.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Popovic, V. (2003). Expert and novice users model and their application to the design process. In Journal of the Asian Design International Conference. Japan: University of Tsukuba.Google Scholar
  31. Schön, D. (1983). The reflective practitioner: How professionals think in action. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
  32. Shah, J. J., Millsap, R. E., Woodward, J., & Smith, S. M. (2012). Applied tests of design skills—Part 1: Divergent thinking. Journal of Mechanical Design, 134(2), 689–698.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Tomasi, D., & Volkow, N. D. (2010). Functional connectivity density mapping. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 107(21), 9885–9890.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Tucker, R., & Abbasi, N. (2012). Conceptualizing teamwork and group-work in architecture and related design disciplines. In ASA 2012 : Building on knowledge, theory and practice : Proceedings of the 46th Annual Conference of the Architectural Science Association, Architectural Science Association, Gold Coast, Qld. (pp. 1–8).Google Scholar
  35. Valkenburg, R., & Dorst, K. (1998). The reflective practice of design teams. Design Studies, 19(3), 249–271.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Wang, J., & Zheng, N. (2013). A novel fractal image compression scheme with block classification and sorting based on Pearson’s correlation coefficient. IEEE Transactions on Image Processing, 22(9), 3690–3702.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Williams, S. D. (2004). Personality, attitude, and leader influences on divergent thinking and creativity in organizations. European Journal of Innovation Management, 7(3), 187–204.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Wu, W. J., & Xu, Y. (2010). Correlation analysis of visual verb’s subcategorization based on Pearson’s correlation coefficient, International Conference on Machine Learning and Cybernetics (ICMLC) (pp. 2042–2046).Google Scholar
  39. Xiao, B., Georgiou, P., Baucom, B., & Narayanan, S. S. (2015). Head motion modeling for human behavior analysis in dyadic interaction. IEEE Transactions on Multimedia, 17(7), 1107–1119.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Nature B.V. 2018

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.School of DesignHunan UniversityChangshaChina
  2. 2.School of Electronic Engineering and Computer ScienceQueen Mary, University of LondonLondonUK

Personalised recommendations