Advertisement

International Journal of Primatology

, Volume 33, Issue 1, pp 246–262 | Cite as

What Role Do Mothers Play in the Gestural Acquisition of Bonobos (Pan paniscus) and Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes)?

  • Christel SchneiderEmail author
  • Josep Call
  • Katja Liebal
Article

Abstract

Contemporary research hypothesizes that biological inheritance and ontogenetic factors shape the development of gestural communication in nonhuman great apes. However, little is known about the specific role that mothers play in the acquisition of their infants’ gestures. We observed 6 bonobo (Pan paniscus) and 4 chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) mother–infant dyads and recorded their gesture types and frequency. We analyzed all behavioral contexts in which gestures occurred as well as the play context alone. Infants of both species were unlikely to share gestures with their mother or unrelated adult females. However, gestural sharing was prevalent within age groups. Within and across species, infant–infant and mother–mother groups were homogeneous regarding the types of gestures they shared, although there was individual variation in the frequency of gesture use. Our findings provide limited evidence that infants learned their gestures by imitating their mothers. Phylogenetic influences seem to be vital in gestural acquisition but, we suggest, repertoire development cannot be disentangled from individual social encounters during life.

Keywords

Bonobo Chimpanzee Communication Gesture acquisition Mother–infant dyad 

Notes

Acknowledgements

We are especially grateful to Apenheul and Burgers’ Zoo (Holland), Dierenpark Planckendael (Belgium), Zoo Leipzig, Zoo Berlin, and Allwetterzoo Muenster (Germany) for their support and friendliness. We thank M. Chase and H. Gretscher for fruitful discussions and comments on earlier drafts of this manuscript. We express a special thanks to R. Mundry for his ideas and time regarding the statistical procedures. We also thank the reviewers for their helpful comments. This study was part of the interdisciplinary research project Towards a Grammar of Gesture, which was funded by the Volkswagen Foundation (Hannover, Germany).

Supplementary material

10764_2011_9570_MOESM1_ESM.doc (124 kb)
ESM 1 (DOC 123 kb)

References

  1. Adams, D. C., & Anthony, C. D. (1996). Using randomisation techniques to analyse behavioural data. Animal Behaviour, 51, 733–738.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Altman, D. G. (1991). Practical statistics for medical research. London: Chapman & Hall.Google Scholar
  3. Altmann, J. (1974). Observational study of behavior: Sampling methods. Behaviour, 49, 227–267.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Bard, K. A. (1992). Intentional behavior and intentional communication in young free-ranging orangutans. Child Development, 63, 1186–1197.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Bates, E., Camaioni, L., & Volterra, V. (1975). The acquisition of performatives prior to speech. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 21, 205–226.Google Scholar
  6. Bruner, J. S. (1981). Intention in the structure of action and interaction. In L. P. Lipsitt (Ed.), Advances in infancy research (Vol. 1, pp. 41–56). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
  7. Bruner, J. S. (1983). Child’s talk. New York: W. W. Norton.Google Scholar
  8. Butterworth, G. (2003). Pointing is the royal road to language for babies. In S. Kita (Ed.), Pointing: Where language, culture, and cognition meet (pp. 9–33). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  9. Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2007a). Comparing the gestures of apes and monkeys. In J. Call & M. Tomasello (Eds.), The gestural communication of apes and monkeys (pp. 197–220). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  10. Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2007b). The gestural communication of apes and monkeys. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  11. Cartmill, E. A. (2008). Gestural communication in orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus and Pongo abelii): A cognitive approach. St. Andrews: University of St. Andrews.Google Scholar
  12. Cavalli-Sforza, L. L., & Feldman, M. W. (1981). Cultural transmission and evolution: A quantitative approach. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
  13. Dice, L. R. (1945). Measures of the amount of ecologic association between species. Ecology, 26, 297–302.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Enquist, M., Strimling, P., Eriksson, K., Laland, K., & Sjostrand, J. (2010). One cultural parent makes no culture. Animal Behaviour, 79, 1353–1362.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Genty, E., Breuer, T., Hobaiter, C., & Byrne, R. W. (2009). Gestural communication of the gorilla (Gorilla gorilla): Repertoire, intentionality and possible origins. Animal Cognition, 12, 527–546.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Gomm, R., Hammersley, M., & Foster, P. (2000). Case study method. London: SAGE.Google Scholar
  17. Haccou, P., & Meelis, E. (1994). Statistical analyses of behavioural data. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  18. Hirata, S., & Celli, M. L. (2003). Role of mothers in the acquisition of tool-use behaviours by captive infant chimpanzees. Animal Cognition, 6, 235–244.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Hobaiter, C., & Byrne, R. W. (2011). The gestural repertoire of the wild chimpanzee. Animal Cognition, 14, 745–767.Google Scholar
  20. Hoff, M. P., Nadler, R. D., & Maple, T. L. (1981). Development of infant independence in a captive group of lowland gorillas. Developmental Psychobiology, 14, 251–265.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Jaeggi, A. V., Dunkel, L. P., van Noordwijk, M. A., Wich, S. A., Sura, A. A. L., & van Schaik, C. P. (2010). Social learning of diet and foraging skills by wild immature Bornean orangutans: Implications for culture. American Journal of Primatology, 72, 62–71.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. King, B. J. (2004). The dynamic dance: Nonvocal communication in African great apes. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  23. Liebal, K., & Call, J. (2012). The origins of nonhuman primates' manual gestures. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 367, 118–128.Google Scholar
  24. Liebal, K., Pika, S., & Tomasello, M. (2006). Gestural communication of orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus). Gesture, 6, 1–38.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Liszkowski, U. (2008). Before L1: A differentiated perspective on infant gestures. Gesture, 8, 180–196.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Lonsdorf, E. V. (2006). What is the role of mothers in the acquisition of termite-fishing behaviors in wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii)? Animal Cognition, 9, 36–46.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Maestripieri, D., & Call, J. (1996). Mother-infant communication in primates. Advances in the Study of Behavior, 25, 613–642.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Manly, B. F. J. (1997). Randomization, bootstrap and Monte Carlo methods in biology. New York: Chapman & Hall.Google Scholar
  29. Mason, W. A. (1963). The effects of environmental restriction on the social development of rhesus monkeys. In C. H. Southwick (Ed.), Primate social behaviour (pp. 161–173). New York: Van Nostrand.Google Scholar
  30. Masur, E. F. (1980). The development of communicative gestures in mother-infant interactions. Papers and Reports on Child Language Development, 19, 121–128.Google Scholar
  31. Pereira, M. E., & Fairbanks, L. A. (2002). Juvenile primates: Life history, development, and behavior. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
  32. Pika, S., Liebal, K., & Tomasello, M. (2003). Gestural communication in young gorillas (Gorilla gorilla): Gestural repertoire, learning, and use. American Journal of Primatology, 60, 95–111.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Pika, S., Liebal, K., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2005a). The gestural communication of apes. Gesture, 5, 39–54.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Pika, S., Liebal, K., & Tomasello, M. (2005b). Gestural communication in subadult bonobos (Pan paniscus): Repertoire and use. American Journal of Primatology, 65, 39–61.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Plooij, F. X. (1978). Some basic traits of language in wild chimpanzees? In A. Lock (Ed.), Action, gesture and symbol: The emergence of language (pp. 111–131). London: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  36. Plooij, F. X. (1984). The behavioral development of free-living chimpanzee babies and infants. Norwood, NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
  37. R Development Core Team. (2009). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing.Google Scholar
  38. Redshaw, M., & Locke, K. (1976). The development of play and social behaviour in two lowland gorilla infants. Journal of the Jersey Wildlife Preservation Trust, 13th Annual Report, 71–86.Google Scholar
  39. Rogers, L. J., & Kaplan, G. (2000). Songs, roars, and rituals: Communication in birds, mammals, and other animals. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  40. Schneider, C., Call, J., & Liebal, K. (2011). Onset and early use of gestural communication in nonhuman great apes. American Journal of Primatology, 73, 1–12.Google Scholar
  41. Smith, W. J. (1977). The behavior of communicating: An ethological approach. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  42. Sokal, R. R., & Rohlf, F. J. (1995). Biometry: The principles and practice of statistics in biological research (3rd ed.). New York: W. H. Freeman.Google Scholar
  43. Tomasello, M., & Call, J. (1997). Primate cognition. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  44. Tomasello, M., Call, J., Nagell, K., Olguin, R., & Carpenter, M. (1994). The learning and use of gestural signals by young chimpanzees: A trans-generational study. Primates, 35, 137–154.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Tomasello, M., Call, J., Warren, J., Frost, G. T., Carpenter, M., & Nagell, K. (1997). The ontogeny of chimpanzee gestural signals: A comparison across groups and generations. Evolution of Communication, 1, 223–259.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Tomasello, M., George, B. L., Kruger, A. C., Farrar, M. J., & Evans, A. (1985). The development of gestural communication in young chimpanzees. Journal of Human Evolution, 14, 175–186.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Tomasello, M., Gust, D., & Frost, G. T. (1989). A longitudinal investigation of gestural communication in young chimpanzees. Primates, 30, 35–50.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. van Lawick-Goodall, J. (1967). Mother-offspring relationships in free-ranging chimpanzees. In D. Morris (Ed.), Primate ethology (pp. 287–346). London: Weidenfeld and Nicolson.Google Scholar
  49. van Schaik, C. (2004). Among orangutans: Red apes and the rise of human culture. Cambridge, MA: The Belknap Press of Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  50. Watts, D. P., & Pusey, A. E. (2002). Behavior of juvenile and adolescent great apes. In M. E. Pereira & L. A. Fairbanks (Eds.), Juvenile primates: Life history, development, and behavior (pp. 148–167). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  51. Wetherby, A. M., Cain, D. H., Yonclas, D. G., & Walker, V. G. (1988). Analysis of intentional communication of normal children from the prelinguistic to the multiword stage. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 31, 240–252.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  52. Wich, S. A., Utami-Atmoko, S. S., Mitra Setia, T., Rijksen, H. D., Schürmann, C., van Hooff, J. A. R. A. M., & van Schaik, C. P. (2004). Life history of wild Sumatran orangutans (Pongo abelii). Journal of Human Evolution, 47, 385–398.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Zentall, T. R. (2006). Imitation: Definitions, evidence, and mechanisms. Animal Cognition, 9, 335–353.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2011

Authors and Affiliations

  • Christel Schneider
    • 1
    • 2
    • 3
    Email author
  • Josep Call
    • 4
  • Katja Liebal
    • 1
    • 2
    • 3
  1. 1.Department of Evolutionary PsychologyFreie Universität BerlinBerlinGermany
  2. 2.Department of Developmental and Comparative PsychologyMax Planck Institute for Evolutionary AnthropologyLeipzigGermany
  3. 3.Department of PsychologyUniversity of PortsmouthPortsmouthUK
  4. 4.Department of Developmental and Comparative PsychologyMax Planck Institute for Evolutionary AnthropologyLeipzigGermany

Personalised recommendations