International Journal of Primatology

, Volume 31, Issue 4, pp 539–551

Sex Differences in the Song of Indri indri

  • Cristina Giacoma
  • Viviana Sorrentino
  • Clement Rabarivola
  • Marco Gamba
Article

Abstract

In some primate species, males and females within a social group emit loud calls in a coordinated manner or chorus. Indri indri emits a very conspicuous loud call that elicits the loud calls of neighboring groups. Previous investigations have hypothesized that the main functions of the indri chorus are related to territorial announcement, intergroup avoidance, and group cohesion. We investigated sex differences in indri song. We recorded and analysed songs given by 10 different groups over 160 d. Overall singing duration did not vary between the sexes. However, males emitted significantly fewer but longer notes. Adult males and females of each group participated in the song with sex-specific repertoires. Females had a song repertoire of 8 note types; males shared all of their 6 notes with females. Apart from the initial roars, in all note types shared by both sexes, male notes were significantly longer than female ones, whereas variations in frequency parameters differed according to the note type. These findings suggest that indri song may provide cues to conspecifics, such as group size and sex composition, which could influence interactions between groups.

Keywords

acoustic communication fundamental frequency loud calls sexual dimorphism strepsirrhine vocalizations 

References

  1. Benz, J. J., French, J. A., & Leger, D. W. (1990). Sex differences in vocal structure in a callitrichid primate, Leontopithecus rosalia. American Journal of Primatology, 21, 257–264.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Boehm, C. (1999). Hierarchy in the forest: the evolution of egalitarian behavior. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  3. Boersma, P., & Weenink, D. (2008). Praat: Doing phonetics by computer (computer program). Retrieved from http://www.praat.org.
  4. Cournac, L., Dubois, M. A., & Riera, B. (2003). Rapid methods for characterizing forest structure in Madagascar. In S. M. Goodman & J. P. Benstead (Eds.), The natural history of Madagascar (pp. 96–103). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
  5. Darwin, C. (1871). The descent of man and selection in relation to sex. London: John Murray.Google Scholar
  6. Deputte, B. L. (1973). Étude d’un type de comportement vocal chez un groupe cap-tif de mangabeys (Cercocebus albigena). In Méthode télémetrique d’enregistrement individuel des vocalisations. D.E.A. Biologie Animale, Eco-Ethologie, Université de Rennes I.Google Scholar
  7. Deputte, B. L. (1982). Duetting in male and female songs of the white-cheeked gibbon (Hylobates concolor leucogenys). In C. T. Snowdon, C. H. Brown, & M. R. Petersen (Eds.), Primate communication (pp. 67–93). Cambridge: Cambridge UniversityPress.Google Scholar
  8. De Vore, I., & Hall, K. R. L. (1965). Baboon ecology. In I. de Vore (Ed.), Primate behaviour: Field studies of monkeys and apes. New York: Holt, Reinhart and Winston.Google Scholar
  9. Dixson, A. F. (1998). Primate sexuality. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  10. Ey, E., Hammerschmidt, K., Seyfarth, R. M., & Fischer, J. (2007a). Age- and sex-related variations in clear calls of Papio ursinus. International Journal of Primatology, 28, 947–960.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Ey, E., Pfefferle, D., & Fischer, J. (2007b). Do age- and sex-related variations reliably reflect body size in non-human primate vocalizations? A review. Primates, 48, 253–267.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  12. Favaro, L., Gamba, M., Sorrentino, V., Torti, V., & Giacoma, C. (2008). Singers in the forest: Acoustic structure of indri’s loud calls and first evidence of vocal tract tuning in a prosimian primate. 35° convegno nazionale dell’Associazione Italiana di Acustica 2008.Google Scholar
  13. Fischer, J., & Hammerschmidt, K. (2002). An overview of the Barbary macaque, Macaca sylvanus, vocal repertoire. Folia Primatologica, 73, 32–45.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Fitch, W. T. (1997). Vocal tract length and formant frequency dispersion correlate with body size in rhesus macaques. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 102, 1213–1222.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  15. Fitch, W. T. (2000). The phonetic potential of nonhuman vocal tracts: Comparative cineradiographic observations of vocalizing animals. Phonetica, 57(2–4), 205–218.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  16. Fitch, W. T., & Hauser, M. D. (1995). Vocal production in nonhuman primates: acoustics, physiology and functional constraints on ‘honest’ advertising. American Journal of Primatology, 37, 191–219.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Gamba, M., & Giacoma, C. (2007). Quantitative acoustic analysis of the vocal repertoire of the crowned lemur. Ethology Ecology & Evolution, 19, 323–343.Google Scholar
  18. Garbutt, N. (1999). Mammals of Madagascar. New Haven and London: Yale UniversityPress.Google Scholar
  19. Geissmann, T. (1993). Evolution of communication in gibbons (Hylobatidae). Ph.D. thesis, Anthropological Institute, Philosophical Faculty II, Zürich University. (English text, German summary).Google Scholar
  20. Geissmann, T. (2002). Duet-splitting and the evolution of gibbon songs. Biological Reviews, 77, 57–76.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  21. Geissmann, T., & Mütschler, T. (2006). Diurnal distribution of loud calls in sympatric wild indri (Indri indri) and ruffed lemurs (Varecia variegata): implications for call functions. Primates, 47, 393–396.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  22. Geissmann, T., & Nijman, V. (2006). Calling in wild silvery gibbons (Hylobates moloch) in Java (Indonesia): behavior, phylogeny and conservation. American Journal of Primatology, 68, 1–19.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  23. Ghazanfar, D., Smith-Rohrberg, D., Pollen, A., & Hauser, M. D. (2002). Temporal cues in the antiphonal calling behaviour of cotton-top tamarins. Animal Behaviour, 64, 427–438.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Green, S. M. (1981). Sex differences and age gradations in vocalizations of Japanese and lion-tailed monkeys (Macaca fuscata and Macaca silenus). American Zoologist, 21(1), 165–183.Google Scholar
  25. Hauser, M. D. (1993). The evolution of nonhuman primate vocalizations: Effects of phylogeny, body weight and social context. American Naturalist, 142, 528–542.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  26. Hewitt, G., MacLarnon, A., & Jones, K. E. (2002). The function of laryngeal air sac in Primates: a new hypothesis. Folia Primatologica, 73(2–3), 70–94.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Heymann, E. W. (1987). Behavior and communication of moustached tamarins Saguinus mystax mystax (Primates: Callitrichidae), in an outdoor enclosure. I. The physical structure of long calls of the moustached tamarin. Primate Report, 17, 45–52.Google Scholar
  28. Koffi Bene, J. C., & Zuberbüler, K. (2009). Sex differences in the use of vocalizations in wild olive Colobus monkeys. European Journal of Scientific Research, 25(2), 267–280.Google Scholar
  29. Konrad, R., & Geissmann, T. (2006). Vocal diversity and taxonomy of Nomascus in Cambodia. International Journal of Primatology, 27(3), 713–743.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Locke, J. L., & Hauser, M. D. (1999). Sex and status effects on primate volubility: clues to the origin of vocal languages? Evolution and Human Behaviour, 20, 151–158.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Marler, P., & Tenaza, R. (1977). Signaling behavior of apes with special reference to vocalization. In T. A. Sebeok (Ed.), How animals communicate (pp. 965–1033). Bloomington: Indiana University Press.Google Scholar
  32. Marshall, J. T., Ross, B. A., & Chantharojvong, S. (1972). The species of gibbons in Thailand. Journal of Mammalogy, 53, 479–486.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Masataka, N. (1987). The perception of sex-specificity in long calls of the tamarin (Saguinus labiatus). Ethology, 76, 56–64.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Mendez-Cardenas, M., Randriananbinina, B., Rabesandratana, A., Rasoloharijaona, S., & Zimmermann, E. (2008). Geographic variation in loud calls of sportive lemurs (Lepilemur spp.) and their implications for conservation. American Journal of Primatology, 70, 1–11.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Milne-Edwards, H., & Grandidier, A. (1875). Histoire physique, naturelle, et politique de Madagascar. Paris: Imprimerie nationale.Google Scholar
  36. Mitani, J. C., & Stuht, J. (1998). The evolution of nonhuman primate loud calls: acoustic adaptation for long-distance propagation. Primates, 39, 171–182.Google Scholar
  37. Mittermeier, R. A., Konstant, W. R., Hawkins, F., Louis, E. E., Langrand, O., Ratsimbazafy, J., et al. (2006). Lemurs of Madagascar (2nd ed.). Washington: Conservation International.Google Scholar
  38. Müller, A. E., & Anzenberger, G. (2002). Duetting in the Titi monkey Callicebus cupreus: Structure, pair specificity and development of duets. Folia Primatologica, 73(2–3), 104–115.Google Scholar
  39. Norcross, J. L., & Newman, J. D. (1993). Context and sex-specific differences in the acoustic structure of common marmoset phee calls (Callithrix jacchus). American Journal of Primatology, 30, 37–54.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Norcross, J. L., Newman, J. D., & Cofrancesco, L. M. (1999). Context and sex differences exist in the acoustic structure of phee calls by newly paired common marmosets (Callithrix jacchus). American Journal of Primatology, 49, 165–181.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  41. Patel, E. R., & Owren, M. J. (2007). Acoustics and behavioral contexts of “gecker” vocalizations in young rhesus macaques (Macaca mullata). Journal of the Acoustic Society of America, 121, 575–585.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Petter, J. J. (1962). Recherches sur l’ecologie et l’ethologie des lemuriens malagache. Memoires de Museum National d’Histoire Natural (Paris), 27, 1–46.Google Scholar
  43. Petter, J. J., Albignac, R., & Rumpler, Y. (1977). Mammifères lémuriens. In Faune de Madagascar, 44 (pp. 391–410).Google Scholar
  44. Petter, J. J., & Charles-Dominique, P. (1979). Vocal communication in prosimians. In G. A. Doyle & R. D. Martin (Eds.), The study of prosimian behaviour (pp. 272–282). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  45. Plichta, B. (2005). Akustyk (Computer Program). Retrieved from http://www.msu.edu/plichtab.
  46. Pollock, J. I. (1986). The song of the indris (Indri indri; Primates: Lemuroidea): Natural history, form and function. International Journal of Primatology, 7(3), 225–264.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Powzyk, J. A. (1997). The socio-ecology of two sympatric indriids: Propithecus diadema diadema and Indri indri, a comparison of feeding strategies and their possible repercussions on species-specific behaviors. PhD Dissertation, Duke University, Durham, NC.Google Scholar
  48. Powzyk, J., & Thalmann, U. (2003). Indri indri, Indri. In S. M. Goodman & J. P. Benstead (Eds.), The natural history of Madagascar (pp. 1342–1345). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
  49. Rendall, D., Owren, M. J., Weerts, E., & Hienz, R. D. (2004). Sex differences in the acoustic structure of vowel-like grunt vocalizations in baboons and their perceptual discrimination by baboon listeners. Journal of the Acoustic Society America, 115(1), 411–421.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Robinson, J. G. (1979). Vocal regulation of use of space by groups of titi monkeys Callicebus moloch. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 5(1), 1–15.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Seyfarth, R. M., Cheney, D. L., & Marler, P. (1980). Vervet monkey alarm calls: Semantic communication in a free-ranging primate. Animal Behaviour, 28, 1070–1094.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Sorrentino, V., Gamba, M., & Giacoma, C. (2008). Song stereotypy and repertoire size of the indris near Andasibe(Madagascar). In G. Cordoni, I. Norscia, & E. Palagi (Eds.), Proceedings of the XVIII Congress of the Italian Primatological Society (16-18 May, 2007). Folia Primatologica, 79, 123–159.Google Scholar
  53. Sorrentino, V., Gamba, M., & Giacoma, C. (in press). A quantitative description of the vocal types emitted in the indri’s song, In J. C. Masters, M. Gamba, & F. Genin (Eds.), Leaping ahead: Advances in prosimian biology. New York: Springer.Google Scholar
  54. Thalmann, U., Geissmann, T., Simona, A., & Mutschler, T. (1993). The indris of Anjanaharibe-Sud, northeastern Madagascar. International Journal of Primatology, 14, 357–381.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Waser, P. M. (1977). Individual recognition, intragroup cohesion and intergroup spacing: Evidence from sound playback to forest monkeys. Behaviour, 60, 28–74.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2010

Authors and Affiliations

  • Cristina Giacoma
    • 1
  • Viviana Sorrentino
    • 1
  • Clement Rabarivola
    • 2
  • Marco Gamba
    • 1
  1. 1.Department of Animal and Human BiologyUniversity of TorinoTorinoItaly
  2. 2.Université de MahajangaMahajangaMadagascar

Personalised recommendations