Advertisement

International Journal of Primatology

, Volume 26, Issue 2, pp 279–294 | Cite as

Aggression and Reconciliation in Two Captive Groups of Lemur catta

  • Elisabetta Palagi
  • Tommaso Paoli
  • Silvana Borgognini Tarli
Article

Abstract

Kappeler excluded the presence of reconciliation in a free-ranging group of Lemur catta, but a recent study on the same group indicated reconciliation, though with a very long PC duration. We collected data on 2 captive groups of ring-tailed lemurs at the Pistoia Zoo (Italy) in order to shed light on conflict resolution in the species. We investigated the influence of seasonality and of a targeted aggression episode on the occurrence of reconciliation. We collected 164 PC-MC pairs for the A group and 141 for the B group. We performed all analyses at the dyadic level via randomization procedures. Despite the targeting episode, we found no difference in the levels of aggression between the 2 groups. In contrast, just before the onset of the targeting episode, B showed significantly lower rates of affinitive behaviors versus A. Reconciliation occurred in A, whereas it was absent in B. Therefore, we suggest that in B, with the decrease of baseline affinitive interactions associated with the beginning of the targeting episode, the function of postconflict reunions probably stopped working. On the whole, we found that contrasting results were probably related to different seasonal and social conditions (A: breeding/pregnancy season, characterized by higher tolerance rates; B: birth season, characterized by lower tolerance rates). Accordingly, reconciliation should be monitored throughout the different seasonal phases.

Keywords

Lemur catta aggression reconciliation relationship quality conflict management 

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Altmann, J. (1974). Observational study of behavior: sampling methods. Behaviour 49: 227–267.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  2. Arnold, K., and Whiten, A. (2001). Post-conflict behaviour of wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii) in the Budongo Forest, Uganda. Behaviour 138: 649–690.Google Scholar
  3. Aureli, F., Cords, M., and van Schaik, C. P. (2002). Conflict resolution following aggression in gregarious animals: a predictive framework. Anim. Behav. 63: 1–19.Google Scholar
  4. Aureli, F., and de Waal, F. B. M. (eds.). (2000). Natural Conflict Resolution. Berkeley: University of California Press.Google Scholar
  5. Aureli, F., van Schaik, C. P., and van Hoof, J. A. R. A. M. (1989). Functional aspects of reconciliation among captive long-tailed macaques (Macaca fascicularis). Am. J. Primatol. 19: 39–51.Google Scholar
  6. Aureli, F., and van Schaik, C. P. (1991). Post-conflict behaviour in long-tailed macaques: I. The social events. Ethology 89: 89–100.Google Scholar
  7. Aureli, F. (1992). Post-conflict behaviour among wild long-tailed macaques (Macaca fascicularis). Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 31: 329–337.Google Scholar
  8. Aureli, F. (1997). Post-conflict anxiety in nonhuman primates: the mediating role of emotion in conflict resolution. Aggr. Behav. 23: 315–328.Google Scholar
  9. Björnsdotter, M., Larsson, L., and Ljungberg, T. (2000). Post-conflict affiliation in two captive groups of black-and-white guereza Colobus guereza. Ethology 106: 289–300.Google Scholar
  10. Bogart, M. H., Kumamoto, A. T., and Lasley, B. L. (1977). A comparison of the reproductive cycle of tre species of Lemur. Folia Primatol. 28: 134–143.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  11. Budnitz, N., and Dainis, K. (1975). Lemur catta: ecology and behavior. In Tattersall, I., and Sussman, R. W. (eds.), Lemur Biology. Plenum Press, New York and London, pp. 219–235.Google Scholar
  12. Castles, D. L., and Whiten, A. (1998). Post-conflict behaviour of wild olive baboons: II. Stress and self-directed behaviour. Ethology 104: 148–160.Google Scholar
  13. Cheney, D. L., Seyfarth, R. M., and Silk, J. B. (1995). The role of grunts in reconciling opponents and facilitating interactions among adult female baboons. Anim. Behav. 50: 249–257.Google Scholar
  14. Cords, M. (1992). Post-conflict reunions and reconciliation in long-tailed macaques. Anim. Behav. 44: 57–61.Google Scholar
  15. Das, M., Penke, Z., and van Hoof, J. A. R. A. M. (1998). Postconflict affiliation and stress-related behavior of long-tailed macaque aggressors. Int. J. Primatol. 19: 53–71.Google Scholar
  16. de Waal, F. B. M., and van Roosmalen, A. (1979). Reconciliation and consolation among chimpanzees. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 5: 55–66.Google Scholar
  17. de Waal, F. B. M. (1986). Conflict resolution in monkeys and apes. In Benirschke, K. (ed.), Primates: The Road to Self-Sustaining Populations. Springer, Berlin, pp. 341–350.Google Scholar
  18. de Waal, F. B. M. (1987). Tension regulation and nonreproductive functions of sex in captive bonobos (Pan paniscus). Nat. Geographic Research 3: 318–335.Google Scholar
  19. de Waal, F. B. M. (1989). Peacemaking Among Primates. Harvard University Press, Cambridge.Google Scholar
  20. de Waal, F. B. M. 1993. Reconciliation among primates: A review of empirical evidence and unresolved issues. In Mason, W. A., and Mendoza, S. P. (eds.), Primate Social Conflict. Suny Press, Albany, pp. 111–144.Google Scholar
  21. de Waal, F. B. M. (2000). Primates—a natural heritage of conflict resolution. Science 289: 586–590.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  22. Digby, L. J. (1999). Targeting aggression in blue-eyed black lemurs (Eulemur macaco flavifrons). Primates 40: 613–617.Google Scholar
  23. Evans, C. S., and Goy, R. W. (1968). Social behaviour and reproductive cycles in captive ringtailed lemurs (Lemur catta). J. Zool. 156: 181–197.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Fuentes, A., Malone, N., Sanz, C., Matheson, M., and Vaughan, L. (2002). Conflict and Post-conflict behavior in a small group of chimpanzees. Primates 43: 223–235.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  25. Gresse, M., Lacour, M. C., Fornasieri, I., and Roeder, J. J. (1994). Targeting aggression in Lemur fulvus albifrons. In Anderson, J. R., Roeder, J. J., Thierry, B., and Herrenschmidt, N. (eds.), Current Primatology: Social Development Learning and Behaviour. pp. 77–80.Google Scholar
  26. Hofer, H., and East, M. (2000). Conflict management in female-dominated spotted hyenas. In Aureli, F., and de Waal, F. B. M. (eds.), Natural Conflict Resolution. University of California Press, Berkeley, pp. 232–234.Google Scholar
  27. Kappeler, P. M. (1990). Female dominance in Lemur catta: more than just female feeding priority? Folia Primatol. 55: 92–95.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  28. Kappeler, P. M. (1993). Reconciliation and post-conflict behaviour in ringtailed lemurs, Lemur catta and redfronted lemurs, Eulemur Fulvus Rufus. Anim. Behav. 45: 901–915.Google Scholar
  29. Kappeler, P. M., and van Schaik, C. P. (1992). Methodological and evolutionary aspects of reconciliation among primates. Ethology 92: 51–69.Google Scholar
  30. Koyama, N. F. (2001). The long-term effects of reconciliation in Japanese macaques (Macaca fuscata). Ethology 107: 975–987.Google Scholar
  31. Kutsukake, N., and Castles, D. L. (2001). Reconciliation and variation in post-conflict stress in Japanese macaques (Macaca fuscata fuscata): testing the integrated hypothesis. Anim. Cognit. 4: 259–268.Google Scholar
  32. Manly, B. F. J. (1997). Randomization, bootstrap and Montecarlo methods in biology. Chapman and Hall, London.Google Scholar
  33. Mundry, R., and Fisher, J. (1998). Use of statistical programs for nonparametric tests of small samples often leads to incorrect P values: examples from Animal Behaviour. Anim. Behav. 56: 256–259.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  34. Nunn, C. L., and Pereira, M. E. (2000). Group histories and offspring sex ratios in ringtailed lemurs (Lemur catta). Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 48: 18–28.Google Scholar
  35. Palagi, E., Paoli, T., and Borgognini Tarli, S. M. (2004). Reconciliation and consolation in captive bonobos (Pan paniscus). Am. J. Primatol. 62: 15–30.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  36. Palagi, E., Telara, S., and Borgognini Tarli, S. M. (2003). Sniffing behavior in Lemur catta: seasonality, sex, and rank. Int. J. Primatol. 24: 335–350.Google Scholar
  37. Pereira, M. E., and Kappeler, P. M. (1997). Divergent systems of agonistic behaviour in lemuride primates. Behaviour 134: 225–274.Google Scholar
  38. Pereira, M. E. (1993a). Agonistic interaction, dominance relation, and ontogenetic trajectories in ringtailed lemurs. In Pereira, M. E., and Fairbanks, L. A. (eds.), Juvenile Primates. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp. 285–305.Google Scholar
  39. Pereira, M. E. (1993b). Seasonal adjustment of growth rate and adult body weight in ringtailed lemurs. In Kappeler, P. M., and Ganzhorn, J. (eds.), Lemur social systems and their ecological bases. Plenum Press, New York, pp. 205–221.Google Scholar
  40. Rasmussen, D. T. (1985). A comparative study of breeding seasonality and litter size in eleven taxa of captive lemurs (Lemur and Varecia). Int. J. Primatol. 6: 501–517.Google Scholar
  41. Roeder, J. J., Fornasieri, I., and Gosset, D. (2002). Conflict and postconflict behaviour in two lemur species with different social organizations (Eulemur fulvus and Eulemur macaco): A study on captive groups. Aggr. Behav. 28: 62–74.Google Scholar
  42. Rolland, N., and Roeder, J. J. (2000). Do ringtailed lemurs (Lemur catta) reconcile in the hour post-conflict?: A pilot study. Primates 41(2): 223–227.Google Scholar
  43. Samuels, A., and Flaherty, C. (2000). Peaceful conflict resolution in the sea. In Aureli, F., and de Waal, F. B. M. (eds.), Natural Conflict Resolution. University of California Press, Berkeley, pp. 229–231.Google Scholar
  44. Sauther, M. L., Sussman, R. W., and Gould, L. (1999). The socioecology of the ringtailed lemur: thirty years of research. Evol. Anthropol. 8: 120–132.Google Scholar
  45. Schino, G. (1998). Reconciliation in domestic goats. Behaviour 135: 343–356.Google Scholar
  46. Silk, J. B., Cheney, D. L., and Seyfarth, R. M. (1996). The form and function of post-conflict interactions between female baboons. Anim. Behav. 52: 259–268.Google Scholar
  47. Van Horn, R. N. (1975). Primate breeding season: photoperiodic regulation in captive Lemur catta. Folia Primatol. 24: 203–220.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Veenema, H. C., Das, M., and Aureli, F. (1994). Methodological improvements for the study of reconciliation. Behav. Proc. 31: 29–38.Google Scholar
  49. Vick, L. G., and Pereira, M. E. (1989). Episodic targeting aggression and the histories of Lemur social groups. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 25: 3–12.Google Scholar
  50. Wahaj, S. A., Guse, K. R., and Holekamp, K. E. (2001). Reconciliation in spotted hyena (Crocuta crocuta). Ethology 107: 1057–1074.Google Scholar
  51. Watts, D. (1995). Post-conflict social events in wild mountain gorillas (Mammalia, Hominoidea). I. Social interactions between opponents. Ethology 100: 139–157.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. York, A. D., and Rowell, T. E. (1988). Reconciliation following aggression in Patas monkeys (Erythrocebus patas). Anim. Behav. 36: 502–509.Google Scholar
  53. Zar, J. H. (1999). Biostatistical analysis. Prentice Hall, New Jersey.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science + Business Media, Inc. 2005

Authors and Affiliations

  • Elisabetta Palagi
    • 1
    • 2
    • 3
  • Tommaso Paoli
    • 1
    • 2
  • Silvana Borgognini Tarli
    • 2
  1. 1.Centro Interdipartimentale Museo di Storia Naturale e del TerritorioUniversità di PisaItaly
  2. 2.Dipartimento Etologia, Ecologia ed Evoluzione, Unità di AntropologiaUniversità di PisaItaly
  3. 3.Centro Interdipartimentale Museo di Storia Naturale e del TerritorioUniversità di PisaCalci (Pisa)Italy

Personalised recommendations