International Journal of Primatology

, Volume 26, Issue 1, pp 73–103 | Cite as

Influence of Social Context on the Use of Blended and Graded Facial Displays in Chimpanzees

Article

Abstract

Our understanding of social communication and emotional behavior in nonhuman primates has advanced considerably through research over the past half century. Chimpanzee facial displays have typically been described as highly graded communicative signals, but we propose an additional distinction: blended displays. They appear to be morphologically and acoustically similar to the expressions in ≥2 prototypical/parent categories. We describe the facial and vocal communicative repertoire of chimpanzees and examine how they use graded and blended signals in different social contexts. Data from behavioral observations revealed that they used facial displays differently depending on the social context. Specifically, the variability can be explained by 7 factors representing nervousness and distress, agonism, contact reassurance, excitement, greetings, play, and vigilance. Additionally, the use of blended displays was not simply divided between the contexts that elicited the parent types, nor were they used in totally unique contexts. Instead, the data showed that the contextual use of blended displays is primarily correlated with the social contexts that elicited only one of the parent expressions. Thus, the blended displays appeared to reflect conflicting internal motivational states in the sender, instead of expressing features of the external environment. We proffer several possible explanations for how the blended signals may be interpreted by receivers and why they would be contextually associated with only one parent group.

Key words

facial expression communication vocalization behavior graded displays 

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Abramson, A. S, and Lisker, L. (1970). Discriminability along the voicing continuum: Cross language tests. Proceedings of the 6th International Congress of Phonetic Sciences, Prague (pp. 569–573).Google Scholar
  2. Altmann, J. (1974). Observational study of behaviour: Sampling methods. Behaviour 49: 227–265.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  3. Andrew, R. J. (1963a). The origin and evolution of the calls and facial expressions of the primates. Behavour 20: 1–109.Google Scholar
  4. Andrew, R. J. (1963b). Evolution of facial expression. Science 142: 1034–1041.Google Scholar
  5. Aureli, F., Preuschoft, S., Flack, J., and Seres, M. (1999). Personal communication, Yerkes Primate Center Field Station, Lawrenceville, GA.Google Scholar
  6. Bakeman, R., and Gottman, J. M. (1997). Observing Interaction: An Introduction to Sequential Analysis, 2nd edn. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.Google Scholar
  7. Bolwig, N. (1962). Facial expression in primates with remarks on a parallel development in certain carnivores (a preliminary report on work in progress). Behaviour 22: 167–192.Google Scholar
  8. Brown, D. A., and Boysen, S. T. (2000). Spontaneous discrimination of natural stimuli by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). J. Comp. Psychol. 114: 392–400.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  9. Calder, A. J., Burton, A. M., Miller, P., Young, A. W., and Akamatsu, S. (2001). A principal component analysis of facial expressions. Vision Research 41: 1179–1208.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  10. Chevalier-Skolnikoff, S. (1973). Facial expression of emotion in nonhuman primates. In Ekman, P. (ed.), Darwin and Facial Expressions. Academic Press, New York, pp. 11–89.Google Scholar
  11. Clark, A., and Wrangham, R. (1993). Acoustic analysis of wild chimpanzee pant hoots: Do Kibale Forest chimpanzees have an acoustically distinct food arrival pant hoot? Am. J. Primatol. 31: 99–109.Google Scholar
  12. de Waal, F. B. M. (1988). The communicative repertoire of captive bonobos (Pan paniscus) compared to that of chimpanzees. Behaviour 106: 183–251.Google Scholar
  13. Ekman, P., and Friesen W. V. (1975). Unmasking the Face. Prentice Hall, Englewood Cliffs, NJ.Google Scholar
  14. Everitt, B. (1977). The Analysis of Contingency Tables. Chapman and Hall, London.Google Scholar
  15. Fleiss, J. L. (1981). Statistical Methods for Rates and Proportions. Wiley, New York.Google Scholar
  16. Fox, M. W. (1969). A comparative study of the development of facial expressions in canids; wolf, coyote, and foxes. Behaviour 36: 4–73.Google Scholar
  17. Goodall, J. (1968). A preliminary report on expressive movements and communication in the Gombe Stream chimpanzees. In Jay, P. C. (ed.), Primates: Studies in Adaptation and Variability, Holt, Rinehart and Winston, New York, pp. 313–519.Google Scholar
  18. Goodall, J. (1986). The Chimpanzees of Gombe: Patterns of Behavior. The Belknap Press of Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA.Google Scholar
  19. Green, S. (1975). Variation of vocal pattern with social situation in the Japanese monkey (Macaca fuscata): A field study. In L. A. Rosenblum (ed.), Primate Behavior. Academic Press, New York, pp. 1–102.Google Scholar
  20. Hess, U., Banse, R., and Kappas, A. (1995). The intensity of facial expression is determined by underlying affective state and social situation. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 69: 280–288.Google Scholar
  21. Hinde, R. A. (1970). Animal Behaviour: A Synthesis of Ethology and Comparative Psychology, 2nd edn. McGraw-Hill, New York.Google Scholar
  22. Hinde, R. A., and Rowell, T. E. (1962). Communication by postures and facial expressions in the rhesus monkey (Macaca mulatta). Proc. Zool. Soc. London 138: 1–21.Google Scholar
  23. Van Hooff, J. A. R. A. M. (1962). Facial expressions in higher primates. Symposia Zool. Soc. Lond. 8: 97–125.Google Scholar
  24. Van Hooff, J. A. R. A. M. (1967). The facial displays of the Catarrhine monkeys and apes. In Morris, D. (ed.), Primate Ethology, Aldine, Chicago, pp. 7–68.Google Scholar
  25. Van Hooff, J. A. R. A. M. (1971). Aspecten van het social gedrag en de communicatie bij humane en hogere niet-humane primaten. Dissertation, University of Utrecht.Google Scholar
  26. Van Hooff, J. A. R. A. M. (1973). A structural analysis of the social behaviour of a semi-captive group of chimpanzees. In von Cranach, M., and Vine, I. (eds.), Expressive Movement and Non-verbal Communication, Academic Press, London, pp. 75–162.Google Scholar
  27. Lorenz, K. (1941). Vergleichende Bewegungsstudien an Anatinen. J. Fur Ornithol. 89: 194–294, Sonderheft.Google Scholar
  28. Marler, P. (1965). Communication in monkeys and apes. In DeVore, I. (ed.), Primate Behavior, Holt, Rinehart and Winston, New York, pp. 544–584.Google Scholar
  29. Marler, P. (1969). Vocalizations of wild chimpanzees: An introduction. In Carpenter, C. R. (ed.), Proceedings of the Second International Congress of Primatology, Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp. 94–100.Google Scholar
  30. Marler, P. (1976). Social organization, communication and graded signals: The chimpanzee and the gorilla. In Bateson, P. P., and Hinde, R. A. (eds.), Growing Points in Ethology, Cambridge University Press, London, pp. 239–279.Google Scholar
  31. Marler, P., and Hobbett, L. (1975). Individuality in the long-range vocalization of wild chimpanzees. Z. Tierpsychologie 38: 97–109.Google Scholar
  32. Marler, P., and Tenaza, R. (1976). Signaling behavior of apes with special reference to vocalization. In Sebeok, T. (ed.), How Animals Communicate, Indiana University Press, Bloomington, pp. 965–1033.Google Scholar
  33. Mitani, J. (1996). Comparative studies of African ape vocal behavior. In McGrew, W., Marchant, L., and Nishida, T. (eds.), Great Ape Societies, Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp. 241–254.Google Scholar
  34. Mitani, J. C., and Gros-Louis, J. (1995). Species and sex-differences in the screams of chimpanzees and bonobos. Am. J. Primatol. 16: 393–411.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Morris, D. (1957). “Typical intensity” and its relation to the problem of ritualization. Behaviours 11: 1–12.Google Scholar
  36. Nishida, T. (ed.) (1990). The Chimpanzees of the Mahale Mountains. Tokyo University Press, Tokyo.Google Scholar
  37. Parr, L. A. (2001). Cognitive and physiological markers of emotional awareness in chimpanzees. Anim. Cog. 4: 223–229.Google Scholar
  38. Parr, L. A., Hopkins, W. D., and de Waal, F. B. M. (1998). The perception of facial expressions in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Evol. Comm. 2: 1–23.Google Scholar
  39. Parr, L. A., Preuschoft, S., and de Waal, F. B. M. (2002). Afterword: Research on facial emotion in chimpanzees, 75 years since Kohts. In De Waal, F. B. M. (ed.), Infant Chimpanzee and Human Child Oxford University Press, New York, pp. 411–452.Google Scholar
  40. Partan, S., and Marler, P. (1999). Communication goes multimodal. Science 283: 1272–1273.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  41. Preuschoft, S., and Van Hooff, J. A. R. A. M. (1995). Homologizing primate facial displays: A critical review of methods. Folia Primatol. 65: 121–137.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  42. Preuschoft, S., and Van Hooff, J. A. R. A. M. (1997). The social function of ‘smile’ and ‘laughter’: variations across primate species and societies. In Segerstrale, U., and Molnar, P. (eds.), Nonverbal Communication: Where Nature Meets Culture, Erlbaum, New Jersey, pp. 171–189.Google Scholar
  43. Redican, W. K. (1975). Facial expressions in nonhuman primates. In Rosenblum, L. A. (ed.), Primate Behavior, Academic Press, New York, pp. 103–194.Google Scholar
  44. Redican, W. K. (1982). An evolutionary perspective on human facial displays. In Ekman, P. (ed.), Emotion in the Human Face, Cambridge University Press, New York, pp. 212–280.Google Scholar
  45. Rinn, W. E. (1984). The neuropsychology of facial expression: A review of the neurological and psychological mechanisms for producing facial expressions. Psychol. Bull. 95: 52–77.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  46. Seyfarth, R. M., Cheney, D. L., and Marler, P. (1980). Monkey responses to three different alarm calls: Evidence of predator classification and semantic communication. Science 210: 801–803.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  47. Tinbergen, N. (1952). Derived activities: Their causation, biological significance, origin and emancipation during evolution. Q. Rev. Biol. 27: 1–32.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  48. Van Hooff, J. (1970). A component analysis of the structure of the social behaviour of a semi-captive chimpanzee group. Experientia 26: 549–550.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  49. Weigel, R. M. (1979). The facial expressions of the brown capuchin monkey (Cebus apella). Behaviour 68: 250–276.Google Scholar
  50. Yerkes, R. M., and Learned, B. W. (1925). Chimpanzee intelligence and its vocal expression. Oxford Press, Oxford, UK.Google Scholar
  51. Young, A. W., Rowland, D., Calder, A. J., Etcoff, N. L., Seth, A., and Perrett, D. I. (1997). Facial expression megamix: Tests of dimensional and category accounts of emotion recognition. Cognition 63: 271–313.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  52. Zoloth, S. R., Petersen, M. R., Beecher, M. D., Green, S., Marler, P., Moody, D. B., and Stebbins, W. (1979). Species specific perceptual processing of vocal sounds by monkeys. Science 204: 870–873.PubMedGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science + Business Media, Inc. 2005

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Division of PsychobiologyYerkes National Primate Research CenterAtlanta
  2. 2.Department of PsychologyEmory UniversityAtlanta
  3. 3.Department of PsychologyStanford UniversityStanford
  4. 4.Living Links CenterYerkes Regional Primate Research CenterAtlanta

Personalised recommendations