Technology, Knowledge and Learning

, Volume 20, Issue 2, pp 231–248 | Cite as

Flipped Classroom Versus Traditional Textbook Instruction: Assessing Accuracy and Mental Effort at Different Levels of Mathematical Complexity

Original research

Abstract

Flipped classrooms are an instructional technology trend mostly incorporated in higher education settings, with growing prominence in high school and middle school (Tucker in Leveraging the power of technology to create student-centered classrooms. Corwin, Thousand Oaks, 2012). Flipped classrooms are meant to effectively combine traditional and online education by utilizing both in and out-of-class time. Despite positively reported implications of the flipped classroom instructional strategy, there is a deep shortage of literature and data that demonstrate advantages for student learning outcomes. The purpose of this preliminary study with directions for future investigations was to examine flipped classroom instruction versus a traditional classroom; specifically, an instructional video versus traditional textbook instruction to assess accuracy and mental effort at three levels of mathematical complexity. College-level nursing students who require mathematical mastery were used as a pilot test group in anticipation that this experience could be translated for larger data sets of variable age groups. Results indicated that accuracy increased and mental effort decreased with flipped instruction. Using Sweller’s cognitive load theory and Mayer’s cognitive theory of multimedia learning as theoretical frameworks, this study lends insight into designing effective instruction for learning environments that could benefit from a flipped classroom framework.

Keywords

Flipped classroom Mathematics Accuracy Mental effort Nursing students 

References

  1. Atkinson, R. K. (2005). Multimedia learning in mathematics. In R. E. Mayer (Ed.), Cambridge handbook of multimedia learning (pp. 393–408). New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Austin, K. A. (2009). Multimedia learning: Cognitive individual differences and display design techniques predict transfer learning with multimedia learning modules. Computers and Education, 53, 1339–1354.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Baddeley, A. D., & Hitch, G. J. (1974). Working memory. In G. H. Bower (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation: Advances in research and theory (Vol. 8, pp. 47–89). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  4. Beckmann, J. F. (2010). Taming a beast of burden—On some issues with the conceptualisation and operationalisation of cognitive load. Learning and Instruction, 20, 250–264.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Berrett, D. (2012). How “flipping” the classroom can improve the traditional lecture. The Chronicle of Higher Education. Feb 19, 2012. https://chronicle.com/article/How-Flipping-the-Classroom/130857/.
  6. Brown, D. L. (2006). Can you do the math? Mathematic competencies of baccalaureate degree nursing students. Nurse Educator, 31, 98–100.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Bull, H. (2009). Identifying maths anxiety in student nurses and focusing remedial work. Journal of Further and Higher Education, 33(1), 71–81.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Chi, M. T. H., & Glaser, R. (1985). Problem solving ability. In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.), Human abilities: An information processing approach (pp. 227–250). San Francisco: Freeman.Google Scholar
  9. Chi, M. T., Leeuw, N., Chiu, M. H., & LaVancher, C. (1994). Eliciting self-explanations improves understanding. Cognitive Science, 18(3), 439–477.Google Scholar
  10. Clark, R. C., & Mayer, R. E. (2011). E-Learning and the science of instruction: Proven guidelines for consumers and designers of multimedia learning (3rd ed.). San Francisco, CA: Wiley.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Clark, R. C., Nguyen, F., & Sweller, J. (2006). Efficiency in learning: Evidence-based guidelines to manage cognitive load (p. 27). San Francisco, CA: Wiley.Google Scholar
  12. Cooper, G., & Sweller, J. (1987). The effects of schema acquisition and rule automation on mathematical problem-solving transfer. Journal of Educational Psychology, 79(347), 362.Google Scholar
  13. Costello, M. (2010). A comparison of three educational strategies for the acquisition of medication calculation skills among baccalaureate nursing students. Ph.D., Simmons College.Google Scholar
  14. Daniel, R. C., & Embretson, S. E. (2010). Designing cognitive complexity in mathematical problem-solving items. Applied Psychological Measurement, 34, 348–364.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Demetry, C. (2010). Work in progressAn innovation merging “classroom flip” and team-based learning. Frontiers in Education Conference (FIE), 2010 IEEE. 27–30 Oct 2010, Washington, DC. doi:10.1109/FIE.2010.5673617.
  16. Elliott, M., & Joyce, J. (2004). Mapping drug calculation skills in an undergraduate nursing curriculum. Nurse Education in Practice, 5, 225–229.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Fischer, G. H. (1995). The linear logistic test model: Foundations, recent developments, and applications (pp. 131–155). New York: Springer.Google Scholar
  18. Foshay, W. R., & Silber, K. H. (2009). Handbook of improving performance in the workplace, instructional design and training delivery (1st ed.). San Francisco, CA: Wiley.Google Scholar
  19. Funkhouser, C., & Dennis, J. (1992). The effects of problem-solving software on problem-solving ability. Journal of Research on Computing in Education, 24, 338–347.Google Scholar
  20. Garnham, C., & Kaleta, R. (2002). Introduction to hybrid courses. Mar, 8. Learning Technology Center, University of Wisconsin-Milwaukee.Google Scholar
  21. Garrison, D. R., & Kanuka, H. (2004). Blended learning: Uncovering its transformative potential in higher education. The Internet and Higher Education, 7, 95–105.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Gillham, D., & Chu, S. (1995). An analysis of student nurses’ medication calculation errors. Contemporary Nurses, 4, 61–64.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Ginns, P. (2005). Meta-analysis of the modality effect. Learning and Instruction, 15, 313–331.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Goldberg, M., Harvey, J. (1983). A nation at risk: The report of the national commission on excellence in education. Phi Delta Kappan, 14–18.Google Scholar
  25. Halford, G. S., Baker, R., McCredden, J. E., & Bain, J. D. (2005). How many variables can humans process. Psychological Science, 16, 70–76.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Harrell, B. M. (1987). Comparing the effect upon students who are taught nursing math in the classroom with students who do not experience the nursing math course. RIE(Jan), 32.Google Scholar
  27. Hodge, J. E. (2002). The effect of math anxiety, math self-efficacy, and computer assisted instruction on the ability of undergraduate nursing students to calculate drug dosages. Dissertation, West Virginia University.Google Scholar
  28. Jan, S., & Rodrigues, S. (2012). Students' difficulties in comprehending mathematical word problems in language learning contexts. International Researchers.Google Scholar
  29. Jeung, H. J., Chandler, P., & Sweller, J. (1997). The role of visual indicators in dual sensory mode instruction. Educational Psychology, 17, 329–343.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Johar, A. R., & Arrifin, S. R. (2001). Issues in measurement and evaluation in education. Bangi: University Kebangsaan Malaysia.Google Scholar
  31. Keller, W. R. (1939). The relative contribution of certain factors to individual differences in algebraic problem solving ability. The Journal of Experimental Education, 8, 26–35.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Leahy, W., & Sweller, J. (2011). Cognitive load theory, modality of presentation and the transient information effect. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 25(6), 943–951.Google Scholar
  33. Mayer, R. E. (1985). Implications of cognitive psychology for instruction in mathematical problem solving. In E. A. Silver (Ed.), Teaching and learning mathematical problem solving: Multiple research perspectives (pp. 123–138). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
  34. Mayer, R. E. (2005a). Multimedia learning: Guiding visuospatial thinking with instructional animation. In P. Shah & A. Miayke (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of visuospatial thinking. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  35. Mayer, R. E. (2005b). Principles for managing essential processing in multimedia learning: Segmenting, pretraining, and modality principles. In R. Mayer (Ed.), The Cambridge handbook of multimedia learning (pp. 169–182). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Mayer, R. E. (Ed.). (2009). Multimedia learning. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  37. McLeod, J., Fisher, J., & Hoover, G. (2003). Key elements of classroom management: Managing time and space, student behavior, and instructional strategies. Alexandria, VA: ASCD.Google Scholar
  38. Melius, J. (2012). Math anxiety and mathematics self-efficacy in relation to medication calculation performance in nurses. Dissertation, University of North Texas.Google Scholar
  39. Miller, G. A. (1956). The magical number seven, plus or minus two: Some limits on our capacity for processing information. Psychological Review, 63, 81–97.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Moreno, R. (2006). Does the modality principle hold for different media? A test of the method-affects-learning hypothesis. Journal of Computer Assisted learning, 22, 149–158.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Moreno, R., & Mayer, R. E. (1999). Cognitive principles of multimedia learning: The role of modality and contiguity. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91, 358–368.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Morris, S. (2010). A comparison of learning outcomes in a traditional lecture-based versus blended course module using a business simulation with high cognitive load. Dissertation, University of San Francisco, CA.Google Scholar
  43. Mousavi, S. Y., Low, R., & Sweller, J. (1995). Reduction cognitive load by mixing auditor and visual presentation modes. Journal of Educational Psychology, 87, 319–334.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. NAEP. (2009). Nation’s Report Card. Retrieved Mar 15, 2010. http://nces.ed.gov/nationsreportcard/mathematics.
  45. NCTM (2010). Overview: Standards for School Mathematics. Problem Solving. Retrieved Sept 7, 2010. http://standards.nctm.org/document/chapter3/prob.htm.
  46. Paas, F. (1992). Training strategies for attaining transfer of problem-solving skills in statistics: A cognitive load approach. Journal of Educational Psychology, 84, 429–434.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Paas, F., & van Merrienboër, J. J. (1993). The efficiency of instructional conditions: An approach to combine perceived mental effort and performance measures. Human Factors, 35, 737–743.Google Scholar
  48. Paas, F., & van Merrienboër, J. J. (1994a). Instructional control of cognitive load in the training of complex cognitive tasks. Educational Psychology Review, 6, 51–71.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Paas, F., & van Merrienboër, J. J. (1994b). Variability of worked examples and transfer of geometrical problem-solving skills: A cognitive load approach. Journal of Educational Psychology, 86, 122–133.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Paas, F., Renkl, A., & Sweller, J. (2003). Cognitive load theory and instructional design: Recent developments. Educational Psychologist, 38, 1–4.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Paivio, A. (1986). Mental representations: A dual coding approach. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  52. Palumbo, D. (1990). Programming language/problem-solving research: A review of relevant issues. Review of Educational Research, 60, 65–89.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Park, B., Moreno, R., Seufert, T., & Brünken, R. (2010). Does cognitive load moderate the seductive details effect? A multimedia study. Paper presented at the American Educational Research Association Conference, Denver, CO.Google Scholar
  54. Schnotz, W. (2011). Colorful bouquets in multimedia research: A closer look at the modality effect. Zeitschrift fur Padagogische Psychologie, 25, 269–276.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Sweller, J. (1988). Cognitive load during problem solving: Effects on learning. Cognitive Science, 12, 257–285.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. Sweller, J. (1994). Cognitive load theory, learning difficulty, and instructional design. Learning and Instruction, 4, 295–312.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  57. Sweller, J., & Cooper, G. A. (1985). The use of worked examples as a substitute for problem solving in learning algebra. Cognitive and Instruction, 2, 59–89.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  58. Tabbers, H. K., Martens, R. L., & van Merrienboër, J. J. G. (2004). Multimedia instructions and cognitive load theory: Effects of modality and cueing. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 74, 71–81.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  59. Tindall-Ford, S., Chandler, P., & Sweller, J. (1997). When two sensory modes are better than one. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 3, 257–287.Google Scholar
  60. Tucker, C. R. (2012). Blended learning in grades 4–12: Leveraging the power of technology to create student-centered classrooms. Thousand Oaks, CA: Corwin.Google Scholar
  61. Walsh, K. A. (2008). The relationship among mathematics anxiety, beliefs about mathematics, mathematics self-efficacy, and mathematics performance in associate degree nursing students. Nursing Education Perspectives, 29(4), 226–229.Google Scholar
  62. Wong, A., Leahy, W., Marcus, N., & Sweller, J. (2012). Cognitive load theory, the transient information effect and e-learning. Learning and Instruction, 22, 449–457.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  63. Young, J. R. (2002). “Hybrid” teaching seeks to end the divide between traditional and online instruction. Chronicle of Higher Education, 48(28), A33–A34.Google Scholar
  64. Zakaria, M. J. (2002). Relationship between learning approach and problem solving on the topic of fraction. Malaysia: Universiti Kebangsaan Mayalasia.Google Scholar
  65. Zakaria, E., & Yusoff, N. (2009). Attitudes and problem-solving skills in algebra among Malaysian matriculation college students. European Journal of Social Sciences, 8, 232–245.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media Dordrecht 2014

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.University of San FranciscoSan FranciscoUSA
  2. 2.San FranciscoUSA

Personalised recommendations