Advertisement

Role of Interaction in Enhancing the Epistemic Utility of 3D Mathematical Visualizations

  • Hai-Ning Liang
  • Kamran Sedig
Article

Abstract

Many epistemic activities, such as spatial reasoning, sense-making, problem solving, and learning, are information-based. In the context of epistemic activities involving mathematical information, learners often use interactive 3D mathematical visualizations (MVs). However, performing such activities is not always easy. Although it is generally accepted that making these visualizations interactive can improve their utility, it is still not clear what role interaction plays in such activities. Interacting with MVs can be viewed as performing low-level epistemic actions on them. In this paper, an epistemic action signifies an external action that modifies a given MV in a way that renders learners’ mental processing of the visualization easier, faster, and more reliable. Several, combined epistemic actions then, when performed together, support broader, higher-level epistemic activities. The purpose of this paper is to examine the role that interaction plays in supporting learners to perform epistemic activities, specifically spatial reasoning involving 3D MVs. In particular, this research investigates how the provision of multiple interactions affects the utility of 3D MVs and what the usage patterns of these interactions are. To this end, an empirical study requiring learners to perform spatial reasoning tasks with 3D lattice structures was conducted. The study compared one experimental group with two control groups. The experimental group worked with a visualization tool which provided participants with multiple ways of interacting with the 3D lattices. One control group worked with a second version of the visualization tool which only provided one interaction. Another control group worked with 3D physical models of the visualized lattices. The results of the study indicate that providing learners with multiple interactions can significantly affect and improve performance of spatial reasoning with 3D MVs. Among other findings and conclusions, this research suggests that one of the central roles of interaction is allowing learners to perform low-level epistemic actions on MVs in order to carry out higher-level cognitive and epistemic activities. The results of this study have implications for how other 3D mathematical visualization tools should be designed.

Keywords

Mathematical visualizations Interactive visualizations Interaction design Learner-visualization interfaces Epistemic actions Information-based epistemic activities Cognitive tasks Computer-supported thinking 3D spatial reasoning 3D lattice structures 

Notes

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank the reviewers of this article for their valuable comments. This research has been funded by the Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada.

References

  1. Billinghurst, M., & Weghorst, S. (1995). The use of sketch maps to measure cognitive maps of virtual environments. In Proceedings of virtual reality annual international symposium (VRAIS’95) (pp. 40–47).Google Scholar
  2. Charistos, N. D., Teberekidis, V. I., Tsipis, C. A., & Sigalas, M. P. (2003). Design and development of a multimedia educational tool for interactive visualization and three-dimensional perception of vibrational spectra data of molecules. Education and Information Technologies, 8(4), 369–379.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Chen, C. (2004). Information visualization: Beyond the horizon. London, UK: Springer.Google Scholar
  4. Dix, A. J., & Ellis, G. (1998). Starting simple–adding value to static visualization through simple interaction. In AVI’98: 4th international working conference on advanced visual interfaces, L’Aquilla, Italy (pp. 124–134). New York: ACM Press.Google Scholar
  5. Dori, Y. J., & Barak, M. (2001). Virtual and physical molecular modeling: Fostering model perception and spatial understanding. Educational Technology & Society, 4(1), 61–74.Google Scholar
  6. Ericsson, K., & Simon, H. (1993). Protocol analysis: Verbal reports as data (2nd ed.). Boston, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  7. Fiore, S. M., Cuevas, H. M., & Oser, R. L. (2003). A picture is worth a thousand connections: The facilitative effects of diagrams on mental model development and task performance. Computers in Human Behavior, 19, 185–199.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Groth, D. P., & Streefkerk, K. (2006). Provenance and annotation for visual exploration systems. IEEE Transactions on Visualization and Computer Graphics, 12(6), 1500–1510.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Hansen, C. D., & Johnson, C. R. (Eds.). (2005). The visualization handbook. Burlington, MA: Elsevier Butterworth-Heinemann.Google Scholar
  10. Ho, C. H., Eastman, C., & Catrambone, R. (2006). An investigation of 2D and 3D spatial and mathematical abilities. Design Studies, 27(4), 505–524.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Hollan, J., Hutchins, E., & Kirsh, D. (2000). Distributed cognition: Toward a new foundation for human-computer interaction research. ACM Transactions on Computer-Human Interaction, 7(2), 174–196.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Hult, L., Irestig, M., & Lundberg, J. (2006). Design perspectives. Human-Computer Interaction, 21(1), 5–48.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Johnson, C., Moorhead, R., Munzner, T., Pfister, H., Rheingans, P., & Yoo, T. S. (2006). NIH/NSF visualization research challenges report. IEEE Computer Society Press.Google Scholar
  14. Kirsh, D. (1995). The intelligent use of space. Artificial Intelligence, 73, 31–68.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Kirsh, D. (1996). Adapting the environment instead of oneself. Adaptive Behavior, 4(3–4), 415–452.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Kirsh, D. (1997). Interactivity and multimedia. Instructional Science, 25, 79–96.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Kirsh, D. (2003). Implicit and explicit representation. In L. Nadel (Ed.), Encyclopedia of cognitive science. New York, NY: Nature Pub. Group.Google Scholar
  18. Kirsh, D., & Maglio, P. (1994). On distinguishing epistemic from pragmatic action. Cognitive Science, 18, 513–549.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Liang, H.-N., & Sedig, K. (2009). Characterizing navigation in interactive learning environments. Journal of Interactive Learning Environments, 17(1), 53–75.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Lowrie, T. (2002). The influence of visual and spatial reasoning in interpreting simulated 3d worlds. International Journal of Computers for Mathematical Learning, 7, 301–318.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Morey, J., & Sedig, K. (2004). Adjusting degree of visual complexity: An interactive approach for exploring four-dimensional polytopes. The Visual Computer: International Journal of Computer Graphics, 20, 1–21.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Napoli, A., & LE Ber, F. (2007). The Galois lattice as a hierarchical structure for topological relations. Annals of Mathematics and Artificial Intelligence, 49(1-4), 171–190.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Neth, H., & Payne, S. J. (2002). Thinking by doing: Epistemic actions in the Tower of Hanoi. In Proceedings of the twenty-fourth annual conference of the Cognitive Science Society, Fairfax, Virginia, August 8–10 (pp. 691–696).Google Scholar
  24. Palais, R. S. (1999). The visualization of mathematics: Towards a mathematical exploratorium. Notices of the American Mathematical Society, 46(6), 647–658.Google Scholar
  25. Peuquet, D. J. (2002). Representations of space and time. New York, NY: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  26. Presmeg, N. C. (1998). On visualization and generalization in mathematics. In Proceedings of twentieth annual meeting of the North American chapter of the International Group for the Psychology of Mathematics Education (North Carolina) (pp. 23–27).Google Scholar
  27. Salomon, G. (Ed.). (1993). Distributed cognitions (learning in doing: Social, cognitive, and computational perspectives). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  28. Sedig, K. (2008). From play to thoughtful learning: A design strategy to engage children with mathematical representations. Journal of Computers in Mathematics and Science Teaching, 27(1), 65–101.Google Scholar
  29. Sedig, K. (2009). Interactive mathematical visualizations: Frameworks, tools, and studies. In E. Zudilova-Seinstra, et al. (Eds.), Trends in interactive visualisation: A state-of-the-art survey (pp. 112–132). New York: Springer.Google Scholar
  30. Sedig, K., & Liang, H.-N. (2006). Interactivity of visual mathematical representations: Factors affecting learning and cognitive processes. Journal of Interactive Learning Research, 17(2), 179–212.Google Scholar
  31. Sedig, K., Rowhani, S., & Liang, H.-N. (2005). Designing interfaces that support formation of cognitive maps of transitional processes: An empirical study. Interacting with Computers: The Interdisciplinary Journal of Human-Computer Interaction, 17(4), 419–452. Elsevier.Google Scholar
  32. Sedig, K., Rowhani, S., Morey, J., & Liang, H.-N. (2003). Application of information visualization techniques to the design of a mathematical mindtool: A usability study. Information Visualization, 2(3), 142–160.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Sedig, K., & Sumner, M. (2006). Characterizing interaction with visual mathematical representations. International Journal of Computers for Mathematical Learning, 11(1), 1–55.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Shah, P., & Miyake, A. (Eds.). (2005). The Cambridge handbook of visuospatial thinking. New York, USA: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  35. Spence, R. (1999). A framework for navigation. International Journal for Human-Computer Studies, 51, 919–945.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Spence, R. (2007). Information visualization: Design for interaction (2nd ed.). Harlow, UK: Pearson Education Limited.Google Scholar
  37. Stylianou, D. (2002). On the interaction of visualization and analysis: The negotiation of a visual representation in expert problem solving. Journal of Mathematical Behavior, 21, 303–317.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Tergan, S.-O., & Keller, T. (Eds.). (2005). Knowledge and information visualization: Searching for synergies. Berlin: Springer.Google Scholar
  39. Thomas, J. J., & Cook, K. A. (2005). Illuminating the path: The research and development agenda for visual analytics. National Visualization and Analytics Center.Google Scholar
  40. Tory, M. (2003). Mental registration of 2D and 3D visualizations (An empirical study). In Proceedings of IEEE visualization ‘03 (pp. 371–378).Google Scholar
  41. Tory, M., & Möller, T. (2004). Human factors in visualization research. IEEE Transactions on Visualization and Computer Research, 10(1), 72–84.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Tversky, B., & Lee, P. U. (1998). How space structures language. In C. Freksa, C. Habel, & K. F. Wender (Eds.), Spatial cognition: An interdisciplinary approach to representing and processing spatial knowledge (pp. 157–175). Berlin: Springer.Google Scholar
  43. van Wijk, J. J. (2006). Views on visualization. IEEE Transactions on Visualization and Computer Graphics, 12(4), 421–432.Google Scholar
  44. Wickens, C. D., Vincow, M., & Yeh, M. (2005). Design applications of visual spatial thinking. In P. Shah & A. Miyake (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of visuospatial thinking (pp. 383–425). New York, USA: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Wu, H.-K., Krajcik, J. S., & Soloway, E. (2001). Promoting understanding of chemical representations: Students’ use of a visualization tool in the classroom. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 38(7), 821–842.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media B.V. 2010

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.The University of Western OntarioLondonCanada

Personalised recommendations