Advertisement

Human Studies

, Volume 30, Issue 3, pp 255–267 | Cite as

Child’s Play: Anatomically Correct Dolls and Embodiment

  • Talia WelshEmail author
Research Paper

Abstract

Anatomically detailed dolls have been used to elicit testimony from children in sex abuse cases. However, studies have shown they often provide false accounts in young, preschool-age children. Typically this problem is seen as a cognitive one: with age, children can correctly map their bodies onto a doll due to greater intellectual ability to represent themselves. I argue, along with the work of Maurice Merleau-Ponty, that although cognitive developments aid in the ability to represent one’s own body, a discussion of embodiment is required in order to understand the use and abuse of anatomical dolls in forensic interviews. This paper examines these issues and maintains that a better understanding of embodied perception in both adults and children helps show how phenomenology can provide a more nuanced understanding to a troubling ethical and legal problem.

Keywords

Anatomical dolls Change blindness Child psychology Embodiment Ecological psychology Forensic interviews Merleau-Ponty Representation 

References

  1. Bruck, M., Ceci, S. J., & Francoeur, E. (2000). Children’s use of anatomically detailed dolls to report genital touching in a medical examination: Developmental and gender comparisons. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 6, 74–83.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Bruck, M, Ceci, S. J., Francouer, E., & Renick, A. (1995). Anatomically detailed dolls do not facilitate preschoolers’ reports of pediatric examination involving genital touching. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 1, 95–109.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Ceci, S. J., Loftus, E. F., Leichtman, M. D., & Bruck, M. (1994). The role of source misattributions in the creation of false beliefs among preschoolers. International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, 42, 304–320.Google Scholar
  4. Dammeyer, M. D. (1998). The assessment of child sexual abuse allegations: using research to guide clinical decision making. Behavioral Sciences and the Law, 16, 21–34.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. DeLoache, J. S., & Marzolf, D. P. (1995). The use of dolls to interview young children: Issues of symbolic representation. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 60, 155–173.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Dickinson, J. J., Poole, D. A., & Bruck, M. (2005). Back to the future: A comment on the use of anatomical dolls in forensic interviews. Journal of Forensic Psychology Practice, 5(1), 63–74.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Everson, M. D., & Boat, B. W. (1997). Anatomical dolls in child sexual abuse assessments: A call for forensically relevant research. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 11, S55–S74.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Faller, K. C. (2005). Anatomical dolls: Their use in assessment of children who may have been sexually abused. Journal of Child Sexual Abuse, 14(3), 1–21.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Goodman, G. S., Quas, J. A., Batterman-Faunce, J. M., Riddlesberger, M. M., & Kuhn, J. (1997). Children’s reactions to and memory for a stressful event: Influences of age, anatomical dolls, knowledge, and parental attachment. Applied Developmental Science, 1(2), 54–75.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Hass, N. (1995). Margaret Kelly Michaels wants her innocence back. New York Times Magazine, 37–41.Google Scholar
  11. Lamb, M. E., Hershkowitz, Sternberg, K. J., Boat, B., & Everson, M. D. (1996). Investigative interviews of alleged sexual abuse victims with and without anatomical dolls. Child Abuse & Neglect, 20(12), 1251–1259.Google Scholar
  12. Levin, D. T., & Simons, D. J. (1997). Failure to detect changes to attended objects in motion pictures. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 5, 501–506.Google Scholar
  13. Mashberg, T., & Hayward, E. (1995). The fells acre case. Boston Herald, 26–32.Google Scholar
  14. Merleau-Ponty, M. (1988). Merleau-Ponty à la Sorbonne: résumé de cours 1949–1952. Paris: Cynara (Original work presented in 1949–1952).Google Scholar
  15. Merleau-Ponty, M. (1996). Phenomenology of perception. (C. Smith, Trans.). London: Routledge (Original work published 1945).Google Scholar
  16. Neisser, U. (1995). Criteria for an ecological self. In P. Rochat (Ed.), The self in infancy (pp. 17–33). Amsterdam: Elsevier.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Neisser, U. (1988). Five kinds of self-knowledge. Philosophical Psychology, 1(1), 35–59.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Ornstein, P. A., & Haden, C. A. (2002). The development of memory: Toward an understanding of children’s testimony. In M. Eisen, J. Quas, & G. Goodman (Eds.), Memory and suggestibility in the forensic interview. Mahwah, New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Publishers.Google Scholar
  19. Piaget, J., & Cartalis, M. E. (1999). Judgment and reasoning in the child. (M. Warden, Trans.). London: Routledge.Google Scholar
  20. Simons, D. J., & Levin, D. T. (1998). Failure to detect changes in people in real-world interactions. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 5, 644–649.Google Scholar
  21. Thierry, K. L., Lamb, M. E., Orbach, Y., & Pipe, M. E. (2005). Developmental differences in the function and use of anatomical dolls during interviews with alleged sexual abuse victims. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 73(6), 1125–1134.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media B.V. 2007

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of Philosophy & Religion #2753University of TennesseeChattanoogaUSA

Personalised recommendations