Foundations of Science

, Volume 16, Issue 1, pp 67–99

The Role of Conscious Attention in Perception

Immanuel Kant, Alonzo Church, and Neuroscience
Article

Abstract

Impressions, energy radiated by phenomena in the momentary environmental scene, enter sensory neurons, creating in afferent nerves a data stream. Following Kant, by our inner sense the mind perceives its own thoughts as it ties together sense data into an internalized scene. The mind, residing in the brain, logically a Language Machine, processes and stores items as coded grammatical entities. Kantian synthetic unity in the linguistic brain is able to deliver our experience of the scene as we appear to see it. Uniquely, the brain records its own history, synthesizing a Movie-in-the-Brain, called the Noumenal Cosmos. Attempting thereby to represent the actual Universe, this makes for a sovereign brain that governs itself. The brain is domicile of an Ego, with its selfhood at stake at all times. Yet, it can know itself only by its actions, in which it appears as an actor in its own movie. Phenomena enter garbled, as confused apparitions, and must be put in good form using top–down feedback control by Ego, so that each movie frame makes rational sense within the overall context of the Noumenal Cosmos. A stack of frames is processed typically in 40 Hz rhythm with 300 ms process time each, for about 12 in the stack at any time. Successive neural centers are processing the stack in the brain assembly line, based on data from increasingly global receptive fields. Ego stitches together the movie frames, but only the top frame is in consciousness for 25 ms. The top frame contains the whole scene where the Ego makes an appearance as the actor that imposes Kantian synthetic unity on the scene, merely an assembly of grammatical texts, in a system-internal coded process language, fitting the scene into the Noumenal Cosmos. But Ego observes Ego only to the extent permitted by the objectivity rule, only what it does and thinks, not its true face. From the Noumenal Cosmos, the Ego receives grammatical messages in the internal sense code. They are integrated into a whole in the reaction of the Ego to the momentary scene. The voluntary nature of Ego’s decisions is explained, based on its ability to code in advance its own actions sequentially in time, as it sees fit with a view to an orderly Noumenal Cosmos, records of code being arranged spatially in neural structures.

Keywords

Perception Phenomena Inner sense Affect self Percept Transcendental ego Synthetic unity Unitary consciousness Unity of apperception = transcendental unity of self-consciousness Prefrontal cortex PFC Medial PFC MPFC Parietal cortex PC Temporal cortex TC Top–down processing Bottom–up processing Default network DN Environment E \({\subset}\) universe U U-data stream Language L Language system LS (more or less identical with L) Noumenal cosmos NK\({\subset}\) LS, (both L, NK, are thought to be dualistically opposed to U) Pre-linguistic structures Historical records Conceptual–categorical apparatus CA Memory bank MB World map = NK. Working Memory WM~ a kind of precursor or older version of world map 

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Bar M., Kassam K.S., Ghuman A.S., Boshyan J., Schmid A.M., Dale A.M. et al (2006) Top–down facilitation of visual recognition. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United State of America 103(2): 449–454CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Bergson, H. (1907, 1911). Creative evolution. New York: Henry Holt & Co. (English translation of L’Évolution créatrice).Google Scholar
  3. Boehler C. N., Schoenfeld M. A., Heinze H.-J., Hopf J.-M. (2008) Rapid recurrent processing gates awareness in primary visual cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United State of America 105(25): 8742–8747CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Brook A. (2006) Kant A unified representational base for all consciousness. In: Kriegel U., Williford K. (eds) Self-representational approaches to consciousness. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, pp 89–110Google Scholar
  5. Buckner R. L., Carroll D. C. (2007) Self-projection and the brain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 11(2): 49–57CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Burchard H.G.W. (2005) Symbolic languages and natural structures, a mathematician’s account of empiricism. Foundations of Science 10(2): 153–245 [Mathematical Reviews MR2193090]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Cashmore A. R. (2010) The Lucretian swerve: the biological basis of human behavior and the criminal justice system. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United State of America 107(10): 4499–4504CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Christoff K., Gordon A. M., Smallwood J., Smith R., Schooler J. W. (2009) Experience sampling during fMRI reveals default network and executive system contributions to mind wandering. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United State of America 106(21): 8719–8724CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Damasio A. R. (1994) Descartes’ error: Emotion, reason, and the human brain. Putnam, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  10. Denise Ingebo-Barth. (2000). The conscious stream. Universal Publisher.Google Scholar
  11. Dennett D.C. (1991) Consciousness explained. Little, Brown and Co., BostonGoogle Scholar
  12. Desimone R., Duncan J. (1995) Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention. Annual review of neuroscience 18: 193–222CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Di Lollo V., Enns J. T., Rensink R. A. (2000) Competition for consciousness among visual events: The psychophysics of reentrant visual processes. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 129(4): 481–507CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Donchin E., Coles M. G. H. (1988) Is the P300 component a manifestation of context updating?. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11: 357–374CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Edelman G. M. (2003) Naturalizing consciousness: a theoretical framework. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United State of America 100(9): 5520–5524CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Engel A. K., Singer W. (2001) Temporal binding and the neural correlates of sensory awareness. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 5: 16–25CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Forstmann, B. U., Anwander, A., Schäfer, A., Neumann, J., Brown, S., Wagenmakers, E.-J., et al. (2010). Cortico-striatal connections predict control over speed and accuracy in perceptual decision making. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United State of America early-bird, August 23. www.pnas.org/cgi/doi/10.1073/pnas.1004932107
  18. Gonsalvez, C., & Barry, R. (2006). Target-to-target interval effects on P300: implications for expectancy theory, Abstract. In 16th Annual conference of the Australasian society for psychophysiology. Canberra: ANU.Google Scholar
  19. Gusnard D. A. (2005) Being a self: considerations from functional imaging. Consciousness and Cognition 14: 679–697CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Gusnard D. A., Akbudak E., Shulman G. L., Raichle M. E. (2001) Medial prefrontal cortex and self-referential mental activity: Relation to a default mode of brain function. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United State of America 98(7): 4259–4264CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Gusnard D.A., Ollinger J.M., Shulman G.L., Cloninger C.R., Price J.L., Van Essen D.C. et al (2003) Persistence and brain circuitry. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United State of America 100(6): 3479–3484CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Gusnard D. A., Raichle M. E. (2001) Searching for a baseline: functional imaging and the resting human brain. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 2(10): 685–694CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Iglói K., Doeller C. F., Berthoz A., Rondi-Reig L., Burgess N. (2010) Lateralized human hippocampal activity predicts navigation based on sequence or place memory. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United State of America 107(32): 14466–14471CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Ingvar D. H. (1985) Memory of the future: an essay on the temporal organization of conscious awareness. Human neurobiology 4: 127–136Google Scholar
  25. Kandel E. (2000) The molecular biology of memory storage: a dialog between genes and synapses. In: Jörnvall H. (Ed.) Nobel lectures, physiology or medicine 1996–2000. World Scientific, SingaporeGoogle Scholar
  26. Kant, I. (1756, 1770). De Mundi Sensibilis atque Intelligibilis Forma et Principiis.Google Scholar
  27. Kant, I. (1781, 1787). Critique of pure reason. Author’s English rendition, using translations by F. Max Müller, as revised by Marcus Weigelt, and by Norman Kemp Smith.Google Scholar
  28. Kleene S. C. (1952) Introduction to metamathematics. Van Nostrand, PrincetonGoogle Scholar
  29. Kosslyn, S. M. (1994). Paperback 1996, Images and brain. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  30. Kriegel, U., Williford, K. (eds) (2006) Self-representational approaches to consciousness. MIT Press, Cambridge, MAGoogle Scholar
  31. Kühn S., Brass M. (2009) Retrospective construction of the judgement of free choice. Consciousness and Cognition 18: 12–21CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Lebrecht S., Badre D. (2008) Emotional regulation, or: how i learned to stop worrying and love the nucleus accumbens. Neuron 59: 841–843CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Libet B. (2004) Mind time, the temporal factor in consciousness. Harvard University Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  34. Matsuhashi M., Hallett M. (2008) The timing of the conscious intention to move. European Journal of Neuroscience 28: 2344–2351CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Marr D., Poggio T. (1979) A computational theory of human stereo vision. Proceedings of Royal Society London B 204: 301–328CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Miller E. K., Cohen J. D. (2001) An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. Annual Review of Neuroscience 24: 167–202CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Muller, H. J. (1929, 1962). The gene as the basis of life. In Proceedings of the international Congress of plant science (Vol. 1, p. 897). Reprinted in Studies in genetics: The selected papers of H. J. Muller, University of Indiana.Google Scholar
  38. Parvizi J., Damasio A. (2001) Consciousness and the brainstem. Cognition 79: 135–159CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Polich J. (2007) Updating P300: an integrative theory of P3a and P3b. Clinical Neurophysiology 118: 2128–2148CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Raichle M. E., Gusnard D. A. (2005) Intrinsic brain activity sets the stage for expression of motivated behavior. The Journal of Comparative Neurology 493: 167–176CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Ridderinkhof R.K., Ullsperger M., Crone E.A., Nieuwenhuis S. (2004) The role of the medial frontal cortex in cognitive control. Science 306(5695): 443–447CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Rose M., Sommer T., Büchel Ch. (2006) Integration of local features to a global percept by neural coupling. Cerebral Cortex 16: 1522–1528CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Schacter D. L., Addis D. R., Buckner R. L. (2007) Remembering the past to imagine the future: the prospective brain. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 8: 657–663CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Schrödinger E. (1943) What is life. Lectures Trinity College, DublinGoogle Scholar
  45. Serre T., Oliva A., Poggio T. (2007) A feedforward architecture accounts for rapid categorization. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United State of America 104(15): 6424–6429CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Snyder, H. R., Hutchison, N., Nyhus, E., Curran, T., Banich, M. T., O’Reilly, R. C., et al. (2010). Neural inhibition enables selection during language processing. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United State of America early-bird, Sep 2. www.pnas.org/cgi/doi/10.1073/pnas.1002291107
  47. Soon C. S., Brass M., Heinze H.-J., Haynes J.-D. (2008) Unconscious determinants of free decisions in the human brain. Nature Neuroscience 11(5): 543–545CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Uddin L. Q., Kaplan J. T., Molnar-Szakacs I., Zaidel E., Iacoboni M. (2005) Self-face recognition activates a frontoparietal mirror network in the right hemisphere: an event-related fMRI study. NeuroImage 25(3): 926–935CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Wager T. D., Davidson M. L., Hughes B. L., Lindquist M. A., Ochsner K. N. (2008) Prefrontal-subcortical pathways mediating successful emotion regulation. Neuron 59: 1037–1050CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Wunderlich K., Rangel A., O’Doherty J. P. (2009) Neural computations underlying action-based decision making in the human brain. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United State of America 106(40): 17199–17204CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Wunderlich K., Rangel A., O’Doherty J. P. (2010) Economic choices can be made using only stimulus values. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United State of America 107(34): 15005–15010CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media B.V. 2010

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of MathematicsOklahoma State UniversityStillwaterUSA

Personalised recommendations