Experimental Economics

, Volume 16, Issue 2, pp 170–189 | Cite as

Self-selection and variations in the laboratory measurement of other-regarding preferences across subject pools: evidence from one college student and two adult samples

  • Jon Anderson
  • Stephen V. Burks
  • Jeffrey Carpenter
  • Lorenz Götte
  • Karsten Maurer
  • Daniele Nosenzo
  • Ruth Potter
  • Kim Rocha
  • Aldo Rustichini
Article

Abstract

We measure the other-regarding behavior in samples from three related populations in the upper Midwest of the United States: college students, non-student adults from the community surrounding the college, and adult trainee truckers in a residential training program. The use of typical experimental economics recruitment procedures made the first two groups substantially self-selected. Because the context reduced the opportunity cost of participating dramatically, 91 % of the adult trainees solicited participated, leaving little scope for self-selection in this sample. We find no differences in the elicited other-regarding preferences between the self-selected adults and the adult trainees, suggesting that selection is unlikely to bias inferences about the prevalence of other-regarding preferences among non-student adult subjects. Our data also reject the more specific hypothesis that approval-seeking subjects are the ones most likely to select into experiments. Finally, we observe a large difference between self-selected college students and self-selected adults: the students appear considerably less pro-social.

Keywords

Methodology Selection bias Laboratory experiment Field experiment Other-regarding behavior Social preferences Prisoner’s dilemma Truckload Trucker 

JEL Classification

C90 D03 

Supplementary material

10683_2012_9327_MOESM1_ESM.pdf (1.3 mb)
(PDF 1.3 MB)

References

  1. Bellemare, C., & Kroger, S. (2007). On representative social capital. European Economic Review, 51(1), 183–202. http://www.elsevier.com/wps/find/journaldescription.cws_home/505541/description#description. CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Belot, M., Duch, R., & Miller, L. (2010). Who should be called to the lab? A comprehensive comparison of students and non-students in classic experimental games. University of Oxford, Nuffield College Discussion Papers (2010-001). Google Scholar
  3. Burks, S., Carpenter, J., Götte, L., Monaco, K., Porter, K., & Rustichini, A. (2008). Using behavioral economic field experiments at a firm: the context and design of the truckers and turnover project. In S. Bender, J. Lane, K. Shaw, F. Andersson & T. von Wachter (Eds.), The analysis of firms and employees: quantitative and qualitative approaches (pp. 45–106). Chicago: NBER and University of Chicago. CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Burks, S., Carpenter, J., & Goette, L. (2009a). Performance pay and worker cooperation: evidence from an artefactual field experiment. Journal of Economic Behavior and Organization, 70(3), 458–469. http://www.elsevier.com/wps/find/journaldescription.cws_home/505559/description#description. CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Burks, S., Carpenter, J., Götte, L., & Rustichini, A. (2009b). Cognitive skills affect economic preferences, social awareness, and job attachment. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science (USA), 106(19), 7745–7750. CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Camerer, C. F. (2003). Roundtable series in behaviorial economics. Behavioral game theory: experiments in strategic interaction. Princeton: Princeton University Press. Google Scholar
  7. Camerer, C. F., & Fehr, E. (2006). When does “economic man” dominate social behavior? Science, 311(5757), 47–52. doi:10.1126/science.1110600. CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Cardenas, J. C. (2005). Groups, commons and regulations: experiments with villagers and students in Colombia. In B. Agarwal & A. Vercelli (Eds.), Psychology, rationality and economic behaviour: challenging standard assumptions. Basingstoke: Palgrave Macmillan. Google Scholar
  9. Carpenter, J., & Seki, E. (2011). Do social preferences increase productivity? Field experimental evidence from Fishermen in Toyama Bay. Economic Inquiry, 49(2), 612–630. http://www.blackwell-synergy.com/loi/ecin/. CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Carpenter, J., Burks, S., & Verhoogen, E. (2005). Comparing students to workers: the effects of social framing on behavior in distribution games. Research in Experimental Economics, 10, 261–290. CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Carpenter, J., Connolly, C., & Myers, C. (2008). Altruistic behavior in a representative dictator experiment. Experimental Economics, 11(3), 282–298. http://www.springerlink.com/link.asp?id=102888. CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Cleave, B., Nikiforakis, N., & Slonim, R. (2011). Is there selection bias in laboratory experiments? Institute for the Study of Labor Discussion Papers (#5488), 1-34. Google Scholar
  13. Eckel, C., & Grossman, P. (2000). Volunteers and pseudo-volunteers: the effect of recruitment method in dictator experiments. Experimental Economics, 3(2), 107–120. http://www.springerlink.com/link.asp?id=102888. Google Scholar
  14. Falk, A., Meier, S., & Zehnder, C. (2012, forthcoming). Do lab experiments misrepresent social preferences? The case of self-selected student samples. Journal of the European Economic Association. Google Scholar
  15. Fehr, E., & Gächter, S. (2000). Cooperation and punishment in public goods experiments. American Economic Review, 90(4), 980–994. CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Fehr, E., & List, J. (2004). The hidden costs and returns of incentives–trust and trustworthiness among CEOs. Journal of the European Economic Association, 2(5), 743–771. http://www.mitpressjournals.org/loi/jeea. CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Fehr, E., & Schmidt, K. (2006). The economics of fairness, reciprocity and altruism—experimental evidence and new theories. In S. Kolm & J. M. Ythier (Eds.), Handbook of the economics of giving, reciprocity and altruism (Vol. 1, pp. 615–691). Amsterdam: Elsevier. Google Scholar
  18. Fischbacher, U. (2007). Z-tree: Zurich toolbox for ready-made economic experiments. Experimental Economics, 10(2), 171–178. http://www.springerlink.com/link.asp?id=102888. CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Fischbacher, U., & Gächter, S. (2010). Social preferences, beliefs, and the dynamics of free riding in public goods experiments. American Economic Review, 100(1), 541–556. http://www.aeaweb.org/aer/. CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Fischbacher, U., Gächter, S., & Fehr, E. (2001). Are people conditionally cooperative? Evidence from a public goods experiment. Economics Letters, 71(3), 397–404. http://www.elsevier.com/wps/find/journaldescription.cws_home/505574/description#description. CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Gächter, S., Herrmann, B., & Thoni, C. (2004). Trust, voluntary cooperation, and socio-economic background: survey and experimental evidence. Journal of Economic Behavior and Organization, 55(4), 505–531. http://www.elsevier.com/wps/find/journaldescription.cws_home/505559/description#description. CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Gächter, S., Nosenzo, D., Renner, E., & Sefton, M. (2012, forthcoming). Who makes a good leader? Cooperativeness, optimism and leading-by-example. Economic Inquiry. doi:10.1111/j.1465-7295.2010.00295.x.
  23. Güth, W., Schmidt, C., & Sutter, M. (2007). Bargaining outside the lab—a newspaper experiment of a three-person ultimatum game. Economic Journal, 117(518), 449–469. http://www.blackwellpublishing.com/journal.asp?ref=0013-0133. CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Harrison, G., Lau, M., & Rutstrom, E. (2009). Risk attitudes, randomization to treatment, and self-selection into experiments. Journal of Economic Behavior and Organization, 70(3), 498–507. http://www.elsevier.com/wps/find/journaldescription.cws_home/505559/description#description. CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Henrich, J., Heine, S., & Norenzayan, A. (2010). The weirdest people in the world? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 33(2/3), 1–75. doi:10.1017/S0140525X0999152X. Google Scholar
  26. Herrmann, B., & Thoni, C. (2009). Measuring conditional cooperation: a replication study in Russia. Experimental Economics, 12(1), 87–92. http://www.springerlink.com/link.asp?id=102888. CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Hoffman, M., & Morgan, J. (2011). Who’s naughty? Who’s nice? Social preferences in online industries. University of California, Berkeley (unpublished manuscript). Google Scholar
  28. Kocher, M., Cherry, T., Kroll, S., Netzer, R., & Sutter, M. (2008). Conditional cooperation on three continents. Economics Letters, 101(3), 175–178. http://www.elsevier.com/wps/find/journaldescription.cws_home/505574/description#description. CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Levitt, S., & List, J. (2007). What do laboratory experiments measuring social preferences reveal about the real world? Journal of Economic Perspectives, 21(2), 153–174. http://www.aeaweb.org/jep/. CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. List, J. (2004). Young, selfish and male: field evidence of social preferences. The Economic Journal, 114, 121–149. CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Long, S. J., & Freese, J. (2006). Regression models for categorical dependent variables using Stata (2nd ed.). College Station: Stata Press. Google Scholar
  32. Patrick, C., Curtin, J., & Tellegen, A. (2002). Development and validation of a brief form of the multidimensional personality questionnaire. Psychological Assessment, 14(2), 150–164. CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Rosenthal, R., & Rosnow, R. (1969). The volunteer subject. In R. Rosenthal & R. L. Rosnow (Eds.), Artifact in behavioral research (pp. 61–118). New York: Academic Press. Google Scholar
  34. Rustagi, D., Engel, S., & Kosfeld, M. (2010). Conditional cooperation and costly monitoring explain success in forest commons management. Science, 330(6006), 961–965. doi:10.1126/science.1193649. CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Stoop, J., Noussair, C. N., & van Soest, D. (2009). From the lab to the field: cooperation among fishermen. Munich Personal RePEc Archive (#28924). Google Scholar
  36. von Gaudecker, H.-M., van Soest, A., & Wengström, E. (2011). Experts in experiments: how selection matters for estimated distributions of risk preferences. Institute for the Study of Labor Discussion Papers (#5575). Google Scholar
  37. Voors, M., Bulte, E., Kontoleon, A., List, J., & Turley, T. (2011). Using artefactual field experiments to learn about the incentives for sustainable forest use in developing economies. American Economic Review, 101(3), 329–333. http://www.aeaweb.org/aer/. CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Zizzo, D. (2010). Experimenter demand effects in economic experiments. Experimental Economics, 13(1), 75–98. CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Economic Science Association 2012

Authors and Affiliations

  • Jon Anderson
    • 1
  • Stephen V. Burks
    • 2
    • 3
    • 4
  • Jeffrey Carpenter
    • 3
    • 5
  • Lorenz Götte
    • 3
    • 6
  • Karsten Maurer
    • 7
  • Daniele Nosenzo
    • 8
    • 9
  • Ruth Potter
    • 1
  • Kim Rocha
    • 1
  • Aldo Rustichini
    • 10
    • 11
  1. 1.Division of Science and MathematicsUniversity of Minnesota MorrisMorrisUSA
  2. 2.Division of Social ScienceUniversity of Minnesota MorrisMorrisUSA
  3. 3.IZABonnGermany
  4. 4.CeDExUniversity of NottinghamNottinghamUK
  5. 5.Department of EconomicsMiddlebury CollegeMiddleburyUSA
  6. 6.Faculty of Business and EconomicsUniversity of LausanneLausanneSwitzerland
  7. 7.Department of StatisticsIowa State UniversityAmesUSA
  8. 8.School of EconomicsUniversity of NottinghamNottinghamUK
  9. 9.CeDExUniversity of NottinghamNottinghamUK
  10. 10.Department of EconomicsUniversity of Minnesota, Twin Cities55455USA
  11. 11.Faculty of EconomicsUniversity of CambridgeCambridgeUK

Personalised recommendations