Experimental Economics

, Volume 12, Issue 1, pp 26–41 | Cite as

Group polarization in the team dictator game reconsidered

  • Wolfgang J. Luhan
  • Martin G. Kocher
  • Matthias Sutter
Article

Abstract

While most papers on team decision-making find that teams behave more selfishly, less trustingly and less altruistically than individuals, Cason and Mui (1997) report that teams are more altruistic than individuals in a dictator game. Using a within-subjects design we re-examine group polarization by letting subjects make individual as well as team decisions in an experimental dictator game. In our experiment teams are more selfish than individuals, and the most selfish team member has the strongest influence on team decisions. Various explanations for the different findings in Cason and Mui (1997) and in our paper are discussed.

Keywords

Experiment Dictator game Team behavior Social preferences 

JEL

C72 C91 C92 D70 

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Andreoni, J., & Petrie, R. (2004). Public goods experiments without confidentiality: a glimpse into fund-raising. Journal of Public Economics, 88, 1605–1623. CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Bohnet, I., & Frey, B. S. (1999). Social distance and other-regarding behavior in dictator games: Comment. American Economic Review, 89, 335–339. CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Bornstein, G., & Yaniv, I. (1998). Individual and group behavior in the ultimatum game: are groups more ‘rational’ players? Experimental Economics, 1, 101–108. Google Scholar
  4. Bornstein, G., Kugler, T., & Ziegelmeyer, A. (2004). Individual and group decisions in the centipede game: are groups more ‘rational’ players? Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 40, 599–605. CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Bosman, R., & van Winden, F. (2002). Emotional hazard in a power-to-take experiment. Economic Journal, 112, 147–169. CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Camerer, C. F. (2003). Behavioral game theory: experiments in strategic interaction. Princeton: Princeton University Press. Google Scholar
  7. Cason, T., & Mui, V.-L. (1997). A laboratory study of group polarization in the team dictator game. Economic Journal, 107, 1465–1483. CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Cooper, D. J., & Kagel, J. H. (2005). Are two heads better than one? Team versus individual play in signaling games. American Economic Review, 95, 477–509. CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Cox, J. C. (2002). Trust, reciprocity, and other-regarding preferences: groups vs. individuals and males vs. females. In R. Zwick & A. Rapoport (Eds.), Advances in experimental business research (pp. 331–350). Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic Publishers. Google Scholar
  10. Cox, J. C., & Deck, C. A. (2006). When are women more generous than men? Economic Inquiry, 44, 587–598. CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Cox, J. C., & Hayne, S. C. (2006). Barking up the right tree: Are small groups rational agents? Experimental Economics, 9, 209–222. CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Davis, D. D., & Harless, D. W. (1996). Group vs. individual performance in a price-searching experiment. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 66, 215–227. CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Davis, J. (1992). Some compelling intuitions about group consensus decisions, theoretical and empirical research and interpersonal aggregation phenomena: Selected examples 1950–1990. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 52, 3–38. CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Dufwenberg, M., & Muren, A. (2006). Gender composition in teams. Journal of Economic Behavior and Organization, 61, 50–54. CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Eliaz, K., Ray, D., & Razin, R. (2006). Choice shifts in groups: A decision-theoretic basis. American Economic Review, 96, 1321–1332. CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Fischbacher, U. (2007). z-Tree: Zurich toolbox for readymade economic experiments. Experimental Economics, 10, 171–178. CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Forsythe, R., Horowitz, J., Savin, N., & Sefton, M. (1994). Fairness in simple bargaining games. Games and Economic Behavior, 6, 347–369. CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Gillet, J., Schram, A., & Sonnemans, J. (2007). The tragedy of the commons revisited: the importance of group decision-making (Working Paper). University of Amsterdam. Google Scholar
  19. Güth, W., Schmittberger, R., & Schwarze, B. (1982). An experimental analysis of ultimatum bargaining. Journal of Economic Behavior and Organization, 3, 367–388. CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Güth, W., Schmidt, C., & Sutter, M. (2007). Bargaining outside the lab: A newspaper experiment of a three-person ultimatum game. Economic Journal, 117, 449–469. CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Kahnemann, D., Knetsch, J., & Thaler, R. (1986). Fairness as a constraint on profit seeking: Entitlements in the market. American Economic Review, 76, 728–741. Google Scholar
  22. Kerr, N., MacCoun, R., & Kramer, G. (1996). Bias in judgment: Comparing individuals and groups. Psychological Review, 103, 687–719. CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Kocher, M. G., & Sutter, M. (2005). The decision maker matters: individual versus group behavior in experimental beauty-contest games. Economic Journal, 115, 200–223. CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Kocher, M. G., & Sutter, M. (2007). Individual versus group behavior and the role of the decision making procedure in gift-exchange experiments. Empirica, 34, 63–88. CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Kugler, T., Bornstein, G., Kocher, M. G., & Sutter, M. (2007, forthcoming). Trust between individuals and groups: groups are less trusting than individuals but just as trustworthy. Journal of Economic Psychology. Google Scholar
  26. Levine, J., & Moreland, R. L. (1998). Small groups. In G. Fisher & G. Lindzey (Eds.), The Handbook of Social Psychology (Vol. 2, 4th edn., pp. 415–469). Oxford: Oxford University Press. Google Scholar
  27. Maciejovsky, B., & Budescu, D. V. (2007). Collective induction without cooperation? Learning and knowledge transfer in cooperative groups and competitive auctions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 92, 854–870. CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Moscovici, S., & Zavalloni, M. (1969). The group as a polarizer of attitudes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 12, 125–135. CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Rockenbach, B., Sadrieh, A., & Mathauschek, B. (2007). Teams take the better risks. Journal of Economic Behavior and Organization, 63, 412–422. CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Stoner, J. (1961). A comparison of individual and group decisions involving risk. Unpublished master thesis, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, Boston. Google Scholar
  31. Sutter, M. (2005). Are four heads better than two? An experimental beauty-contest game with teams of different size. Economics Letters, 88, 41–46. CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Sutter, M. (2007). Are teams prone to myopic loss aversion? An experimental study on individual versus team investment behavior. Economics Letters, 97, 128–132. CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Sutter, M., Kocher, M. G., & Strauß, S. (2006). Individuals and teams in UMTS-license auctions (Working Paper). University of Innsbruck. Google Scholar
  34. Teger, A., & Pruitt, D. (1967). Components of group risk taking. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 3, 189–205. CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Wildschut, T., Lodewijkx, H. F. M., & Insko, C. A. (2001). Toward a reconciliation of diverging perspectives on interindividual-intergroup discontinuity: the role of procedural interdependence. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 37, 273–285. CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Economic Science Association 2007

Authors and Affiliations

  • Wolfgang J. Luhan
    • 1
  • Martin G. Kocher
    • 2
    • 3
  • Matthias Sutter
    • 3
    • 4
  1. 1.Center for Social Science MethodologyUniversity of OldenburgOldenburgGermany
  2. 2.Department of Economics, CREEDUniversity of AmsterdamAmsterdamThe Netherlands
  3. 3.Department of Public FinanceUniversity of InnsbruckInnsbruckAustria
  4. 4.Department of EconomicsUniversity of GöteborgGöteborgSweden

Personalised recommendations