Advertisement

Ethics and Information Technology

, Volume 20, Issue 4, pp 249–263 | Cite as

Real moral problems in the use of virtual reality

  • Erick Jose Ramirez
  • Scott LaBarge
Original Paper
  • 353 Downloads

Abstract

In this paper, we argue that, under a specific set of circumstances, designing and employing certain kinds of virtual reality (VR) experiences can be unethical. After a general discussion of simulations and their ethical context, we begin our argument by distinguishing between the experiences generated by different media (text, film, computer game simulation, and VR simulation), and argue that VR experiences offer an unprecedented degree of what we call “perspectival fidelity” that prior modes of simulation lack. Additionally, we argue that when VR experiences couple this perspectival fidelity with what we call “context realism,” VR experiences have the ability to produce “virtually real experiences.” We claim that virtually real experiences generate ethical issues for VR technologies that are unique to the medium. Because subjects of these experiences treat them as if they were real, a higher degree of ethical scrutiny should be applied to any VR scenario with the potential to generate virtually real experiences. To mitigate this unique moral hazard, we propose and defend what we call “The Equivalence Principle.” This principle states that “if it would be wrong to allow subjects to have a certain experience in reality, then it would be wrong to allow subjects to have that experience in a virtually real setting.” We argue that such a principle, although limited in scope, should be part of the risk analysis conducted by any Institutional Review Boards, psychologists, empirically oriented philosophers, or game designers who are using VR technology in their work.

Keywords

Applied ethics Institutional review boards Media experience Moral psychology Phenomenology Virtual Reality 

Notes

Funding

Funding was provided by Markkula Center for Applied Ethics and Oculus Education Grant.

References

  1. Ahn, S. J., Bostick, J., Ogle, E., Nowak, K., McGillicuddy, K., & Bailenson, J. N. (2016). Experiencing nature: Embodying animals in immersive virtual environments increases inclusion of nature in self and involvement with nature. Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication.  https://doi.org/10.1111/jcc4.12173.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Anderson, C. A., Keio, A. S., Ochanomizu, N. I., Swing, E. L., Bushman, B. J., Rothstein, H. R., et al. (2010). Violent video game effects on aggression, empathy, and prosocial behavior in eastern and western countries: A meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin, 136(2), 151–173.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Berger, J. M. (2009). Replicating Milgram: Would people still obey today? The American Psychologist, 64(1), 1–11.  https://doi.org/10.1037/a0010932.MathSciNetCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Cummings, J., & Bailenson, J. (2016). How immersive is enough? A meta-analysis of the effect of immersive technology on user presence. Media Psychology, 19(2), 272–309.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Di Nucci, E. (2012). Self-sacrifice and the trolley problem. Philosophical Psychology, 26(5), 662–672.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Feinberg, J. (1985). Offense to others. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  7. Felnhofera, A., Kothgassnera, O. D., Schmidt, M., Heinzle, A.-K., Beutl, L., Hlavacs, H., et al. (2015). Is virtual reality emotionally arousing? Investigating five emotion inducing virtual park scenarios. International Journal of Human-Computer Studies, 82, 48–56.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Fischer, J. M., & Ravizza, M. (1998). Responsibility and control: A theory of moral responsibility. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Fischer, P., Kastenmüller, A., & Greitemeyer, T. (2009). Media violence and the self: The impact of personalized gaming characters in aggressive video games on aggressive behavior. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2009.06.010.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Foot, P. (1978). The problem of abortion and the doctrine of double effect. In Virtues and vices and other essays in moral philosophy. Oxford: Clarendon Press.Google Scholar
  11. Fox, J., Bailenson, J., & Ricciardi, T. (2012). Physiological responses to virtual selves and virtual others. Journal of Cybertherapy and Rehabilitation, 5(1), 69–72.Google Scholar
  12. Frigg, R., & Hartmann, S. Models in Science. The stanford encyclopedia of philosophy (Spring 2017 Edition). Edward N. Zalta (ed.), Retrieved fromhttps://plato.stanford.edu/archives/spr2017/entries/models-science/.
  13. Godfrey-Smith, P. (2006). The strategy of model-based science. Biology and Philosophy, 21(5), 725–740.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Goldi, P. (2011). Anti-empathy. In A. Coplan & P. Goldie (Eds.), Empathy: Philosophical and psychological perspectives (pp. 318–330). Oxford: Oxford UP.Google Scholar
  15. Haidt, J., Koller, S. H., & Dias, M. G. (1993). Affect, culture, and morality, or is it wrong to eat your dog? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65(4), 613–628.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Handrahan, M. (2016). VR devs call for restraint on horror games and jump scares. Gamesindustry.biz. Retrieved August 9, 2017 from http://www.gamesindustry.biz/articles/2016-03-14-vr-developers-advise-caution-on-horror-games-and-jump-scares.
  17. Herrera, D. C., & Perry, A. (Eds.), (2013). Ethics and neurodiversity. Newcastle upon Tyne: Cambridge Scholars Publishing.Google Scholar
  18. Higuera-Trujillo, J. L., López-Tarruella, M., & Llinares, M. C. (2017). Psychological and physiological human responses to simulated and real environments: A comparison between photographs, 360° panoramas, and virtual reality. Applied Ergonomics, 65, 398–409.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Hudson, L. (2016). It’s not real, it’s not real, it’s not real: Virtual reality and the dangers of 360-degree abuse. Slate. Retrieved August 09, 2017 from http://www.slate.com/articles/arts/gaming/2016/05/virtual_reality_can_be_more_amazing_and_ore_terrifying_than_a_traditional.html.
  20. Kivikangas, J. M., Chanel, G., Cowley, B., Ekman, I., Salminen, M. J., & Simo, R. N. (2011). A review of the use of psychophysiological methods in game research. Journal of Gaming & Virtual Worlds, 3(3), 181–199.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Knuuttila, T. (2011). Modelling and representing: An artefactual approach to model-based representation. Studies in History and Philosophy of Science Part A, 42(2), 262–271.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Krahé, B., & Möller, I. (2010). Longitudinal effects of media violence on aggression and empathy among German adolescents. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 31, 401–409.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Krohs, U. (2008). How digital computer simulations explain real-world processes. International Studies in the Philosophy of Science, 22(3), 277–292.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Lejacq, Y. (2013). ‘Grand Theft Auto V’ torture episode sparks controversy. News, N. B. C. Retrieved January 08, 2017 from http://www.nbcnews.com/technology/grand-theft-auto-v-torture-episode-sparks-controversy-4B11185864.
  25. Liao, M. S., Wiegmann, A., Joshua, A., & Vong, G. (2012). Putting the trolley in order: Experimental philosophy and the loop case. Philosophical Psychology, 25(5), 661–671.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Madary, M., & Metzinger, T. K. (2016). Real virtuality: A code of ethical conduct. Recommendations for good scientific practice and the consumers of VR-technology. Frontiers in Robotics and AI.  https://doi.org/10.3389/frobt.2016.00003.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. McLay, R. N., Wood, D. P., Webb-Murphy, J. A., Spira, J. L., Wiederhold, M. D., Pyne, J. M., et al. (2011). A randomized, controlled trial of virtual reality-graded exposure therapy for post-traumatic stress disorder in active duty service members with combat-related post-traumatic stress disorder. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking, 14(4), 223–229.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Milgram, S. (1963). Behavioral study of obedience. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 67, 371–378.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Morrison, M. (2009). Models, measurement and computer simulation: The changing face of experimentation. Philosophical Studies, 143(1), 33–57.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Navarrete, C. D., McDonald, M. M., Mott, M. L., & Asher, B. (2012). Virtual morality: Emotion and action in a simulated three-dimensional ‘trolley problem’. Emotion, 12(2), 364–370.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Nichols, S., & Knobe, J. (2007). Moral responsibility and determinism: The cognitive science of folk intuitions. Nous, 41, 663–685.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Parsons, T. D. (2015). Virtual reality for enhanced ecological validity and experimental control in the clinical, affective and social neurosciences. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 9, 650.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Parsons, T. D., Gaggioli, A., & Riva, G. (2017). Virtual reality for research in social neuroscience. Brain Sciences.  https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci7040042.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Parsons, T. D., & Rizzo, A. A. (2008). Affective outcomes of virtual reality exposure therapy for anxiety and specific phobias: A meta-analysis. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 39(3), 250–261.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Pastötter, B., Gleixner, S., Neuhauser, T., & Karl-Heinz, T. B. (2013). To push or not to push? Affective influences on moral judgment depend on decision frame. Cognition, 126(3), 373–377.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Patil, I., Cogoni, C., Zangrando, N., Chittaro, L., & Silani, G. (2014). Affective basis of judgment-behavior discrepancy in virtual experiences of moral dilemmas. Social Neuroscience, 9(1), 94–107.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Pertaub, D. P., Slater, M., & Barker, C. (2002). An experiment on public speaking anxiety in response to three different types of virtual audience. Presence Teleoperators and Virtual Environments, 11(1), 68–78.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Ramirez, E. (2017). Empathy and the limits of thought experiments. Metaphilosophy, 48(4), 504–526.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Riva, G., Mantovani, F., Capideville, C. S., Preziosa, A., Morganti, F., Villani, D., et al. (2007). Affective interactions using virtual reality: The link between presence and emotions. Cyberpsychology and Behavior, 10(1), 46–56.Google Scholar
  40. Rockstar North, et al. (2013). Grand Theft Auto V. PlayStation 3 & Xbox 360: Rockstar Games.Google Scholar
  41. Rosenberg, R., Baughman, S., & Bailenson, J. (2013). Virtual superheroes: Using superpowers in virtual reality to encourage prosocial behavior. PLoS ONE, 8, e55003.  https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0055003.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Sanchez-Vives, M. V., & Slater, M. (2005). From presence to consciousness through virtual reality. Nature Reviews: Neuroscience, 6, 332–339.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Savage, J., & Yancey, C. (2008). The effects of media violence exposure on criminal aggression: A meta-analysis. Criminal Justice and Behavior, 35(6), 772–791.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Schlenker, B. R., & Forsyth, D. R. (1977). On the ethics of psychological research. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 13(4), 369–396.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Shue, H. (1978). Torture. Philosophy and Public Affairs, 7, 124–143.Google Scholar
  46. Skulmowski, A., Bunge, A., Kaspar, K., & Pipa, G. (2014). Forced-choice decision-making in modified trolley dilemma situations: A virtual reality and eye tracking study. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 8, 426.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Slater, M., Antley, A., Davison, D., Swapp, D., Guger, C., Barker, C., Pistrang, N., et al. (2006). A virtual reprise of the Stanley Milgram obedience experiments. PLoS ONE, 1, e39.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Szabó, T. S., & Liao, S. (2016). The problem of imaginative resistance. In J. Gibson & N. Carroll (Eds.), The Routledge Companion to Philosophy of Literature (pp. 405–418). Routledge.Google Scholar
  49. Valkenburg, P. M. & Peter J. (2013). The differential susceptibility to media effects model. Journal of Communication, 63, 221–243.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Williams, B. (1985). Ethics and the limits of philosophy. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  51. Winsberg, E. (2009). A tale of two methods. Synthese, 169(3), 575–592.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Won, A., Bailenson, J., Lee, J., & Lanier, J. (2015). Homuncular flexibility in virtual reality. Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication, 20(3), 241–259.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Nature B.V. 2018

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Philosophy DepartmentSanta Clara UniversitySanta ClaraUSA

Personalised recommendations