Ethics and Information Technology

, Volume 16, Issue 3, pp 251–262 | Cite as

Is the repugnance about betting on terrorist attacks misguided?

Original Paper
  • 458 Downloads

Abstract

Prediction markets designed to predict terrorism through traders’ investments on the likelihood of specific terrorist attacks are, strictly speaking, enabling those traders to bet on terrorism. Betting on terrorist attacks, like some other forms of betting on death, has been accused of being repugnant. In this paper, it is argued that while government-backed effective intelligence-gathering prediction markets on terrorism (PMsoT) might elicit feelings of repugnance, those feelings are likely to be misguided. The feelings of repugnance arise because PMsoT are assumed to be associated with terrorism in encouraging and endorsing ways and with human death in a disrespectful way. However, it is argued that these feelings of repugnance are misguided in the case of government-backed effective intelligence-gathering PMsoT because the purpose and effect of such prediction markets, and (on balance) the trading they encourage, is to prevent and renounce terrorism and show respect for matters of life and death.

Keywords

Prediction markets Terrorism Repugnance Prediction markets on terrorism Policy Analysis Market 

References

  1. Berg, J., Forsythe, R., Nelson, F., & Rietz, T. (2008). Results from a dozen years of election futures markets research. Handbook of Experimental Economics Results, 1, 742–751.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Biever, C., & Carrington, D. (2003). Pentagon cancels futures market on terror. New Scientist, 30 July 2003. Available from http://www.newscientist.com/article/dn4007-pentagon-cancels-futures-market-on-terror.html Accessed on March 21, 2013.
  3. Bush, G. W. (2002). Text of President Bush’s 2002 State of the Union Address. The Washington Post, 29 January 2002. Available from http://www.washingtonpost.com/wp-srv/onpolitics/transcripts/sou012902.htm. Accessed on March 26, 2013.
  4. Conklin, B. A. (2001). Consuming grief. Austin: University of Texas Press.Google Scholar
  5. Courson, P., & Turnham, S. (2003). Amid furor, Pentagon kills terrorism futures market. CNN.com, 30 July 2003. Available from http://edition.cnn.com/2003/ALLPOLITICS/07/29/terror.market/. Accessed on March 21, 2013.
  6. Crawford, C., & Galdikas, B. M. (1986). Rape in non-human animals: An evolutionary perspective. Canadian Psychology, 27(3), 215–230.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Dobbs, L. (2003). Deep-sixing a bright idea. U.S. News and World Report, 11 August 2003.Google Scholar
  8. Haidt, J. (2001). The emotional dog and its rational tail: A social intuitionist approach to moral judgment. Psychological Review, 108(4), 814.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Haidt, J., & Joseph, C. (2004). Intuitive ethics: How innately prepared intuitions generate culturally variable virtues. Daedalus, 133(4), 55–66.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Hanson, R. (2006). Designing real terrorism futures. Public Choice, 128(1), 257–274.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Hanson, R. (2007). The Policy Analysis Market (a thwarted experiment in the use of prediction markets for public policy). Innovations: Technology, Governance, Globalization, 2(3), 73–88.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Ho, T. H., & Chen, K. Y. (2007). Discovering and managing new product blockbusters: The magic and science of prediction markets. California Management Review, 50, 144–158.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Kass, L. (1998). The wisdom of repugnance: Why we should ban the cloning of humans. Valparaiso University Law Review, 32(2), 679–705.MathSciNetGoogle Scholar
  14. Kekes, J. (1986). Moral intuition. American Philosophical Quarterly, 23(1), 83–93.Google Scholar
  15. Kekes, J. (1998). A case for conservatism. New York: Cornell University Press.Google Scholar
  16. LaFree, G. (2010). The global terrorism database: Accomplishments and challenges. Perspectives on Terrorism, 4(1), 24–46.Google Scholar
  17. LaFree, G., Yang, S. M., & Crenshaw, M. (2009). Trajectories of terrorism. Criminology & Public Policy, 8(3), 445–473.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Lazar, A., & Lazar, M. M. (2004). The discourse of the new world order: ‘Out-casting’ the double face of threat. Discourse & Society, 15(2–3), 223–242.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Lieberman, M. (2000). Intuition: A social cognitive neuroscience approach. Psychological Bulletin, 126, 109–137.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Looney, R. E. (2004). DARPA’s Policy Analysis Market for intelligence: Outside the box or off the wall? International Journal of Intelligence and Counterintelligence, 17(3), 405–419.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Meirowitz, A., & Tucker, J. A. (2004). Learning from terrorism markets. Perspectives on Politics, 2(2), 331–336.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Mondimore, F. M. (1996). A natural history of homosexuality. Baltimore: JHU Press.Google Scholar
  23. Myers, D. G. (2004). Intuition: Its powers and perils. New Haven: Yale University Press.Google Scholar
  24. Nelson, T. E., Oxley, Z. M., & Clawson, R. A. (1997). Toward a psychology of framing effects. Political Behavior, 19(3), 221–246.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Pennock, D. (2004). The good side of the ‘Terror Futures’ idea (yes, there is one). Available from http://dpennock.com/pam.html. Accessed on March 14, 2013.
  26. Richey, M. (2005). Thoughts on the theory and practice of speculative markets qua event predictors. Essays in Philosophy, 6(1): Article 26, no page.Google Scholar
  27. Roth, A. E. (2007). Repugnance as a constraint on markets. Journal of Economic Perspectives, 21(3), 37–58.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Schoen, J. W. (2003). Pentagon kills ‘terror futures market. NBCNEWS.com, 29 July 2003. Available from http://www.nbcnews.com/id/3072985/#.UUopfBffA_s. Accessed on March 21, 2013.
  29. Setty, S. (2011). What’s in a name? How nations define terrorism ten years after 9/11. Journal of International Law, 33(1). Available from http://scholarship.law.upenn.edu/jil/vol33/iss1/1. Accessed on July 25, 2014.
  30. Shachtman, N. (2003). The case for terrorism futures. Wired.com, July 30, 2003. Retrieved March 26, 2013, from http://www.wired.com/politics/law/news/2003/07/59818?currentPage=all.
  31. Sinnott-Armstrong, W. (2005). Moral intuitionism and empirical psychology. In T. Horgan & M. Timmons (Eds.), Metaethics after Moore (pp. 339–365). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  32. Sinnott-Armstrong, W. (2008). Framing moral intuitions. In W. Sinnott-Armstrong (Ed.), Moral psychology (Vol. 2, pp. 47–76)., The cognitive science of morality: Intuition and diversity Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  33. Stiglitz, J. (2003). Terrorism: There’s no futures in it. Los Angeles Times, 31 July 2003: B13.Google Scholar
  34. Sunstein, C. R. (2005). Group judgments: Statistical means, deliberation, and information markets. New York University Law Review, 80, 962–1049.Google Scholar
  35. Surowiecki, J. (2004). The wisdom of crowds. New York: Doubleday.Google Scholar
  36. Teichman, J. (1989). How to define terrorism. Philosophy, 64(250), 505–517.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Weijers, D. (2013a). Technology: Prediction markets. Observatory for Responsible Research and Innovation in ICT. Available from http://torrii.responsible-innovation.org.uk/resource-detail/1181. Accessed on August 27, 2013.
  38. Weijers, D. (2013b). Intuitive biases in judgments about thought experiments: The experience machine revisited. Philosophical Writings, 41(1), 17–31.Google Scholar
  39. Wilson-Buford, K. (2013). From exclusion to acceptance: A case history of homosexuality in the US court of military appeals. Journal of Homosexuality, 60(2–3), 250–272.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Wolfers, J., & Zitzewitz, E. (2004). Prediction markets (No. w10504). National Bureau of Economic Research.Google Scholar
  41. Woodward, J., & Allman, J. (2007). Moral intuition: Its neural substrates and normative significance. Journal of Physiology, 101(4–6), 179–202.Google Scholar
  42. Yeh, P. F. (2006). Using prediction markets to enhance US intelligence capabilities. Studies in Intelligence, 50(4), 137–149.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media Dordrecht 2014

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Victoria University of WellingtonWellingtonNew Zealand

Personalised recommendations