European Journal of Law and Economics

, Volume 42, Issue 3, pp 491–513 | Cite as

Do good deeds make bad people?

Article

Abstract

According to the so-called ‘self-licensing effect’, committing to a virtuous act in a preceding choice may lead to behave less virtuously in the succeeding decision. Consequently, well-intentioned policies can lead to overall counter-productive effects by licensing people to behave badly in related behaviors. On the other side, motivational crowding theory argues that constraining people to adopt a desirable behavior can backfire. We use of a classroom experiment to test whether a regulatory framework to incentivize individuals to adopt pro-environmental behavior generate similar spillovers in terms of licensing effect than a non-regulatory framework. We show that the way the good deed is caused doesn’t seem to influence the licensing effect. Nevertheless, we found that business- and environmental-orientated majors react adversely to the regulatory framework. We show that environmental-orientated students exhibit higher intrinsically motivations than business-orientated ones. Accordingly, we suggest that the licensing effect is more likely to arise when the preceding ‘virtuous’ act is freely chosen (respectively regulatory caused) for non-intrinsically (respectively intrinsically) motivated individuals.

Keywords

Environment Regulation Self-licensing Voluntary approaches 

JEL Classification

Q50 D03 D04 K32 

References

  1. Adda, G. (2011). Motivation crowding in environmental protection: Evidence from an artefactual field experiment. Ecological Economics, 70(11), 2083–2097.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Alberini, A., & Segerson, K. (2002). Assessing voluntary programs to improve environmental quality. Environmental and Resource Economics, 22, 157–184.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Bauman, Y., & Rose, E. (2011). Selection or indoctrination: Why do economics students donate less than the rest? Journal of Economic Behavior and Organization, 79(3), 318–327.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Bénabou, R., & Tirole, J. (2006). Incentives and prosocial behavior. The American Economic Review, 96(5), 1652–1678.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Bénabou, R., & Tirole, J. (2011). Identity, morals, and taboos: Beliefs as assets. The Quarterly Journal of Economics, 126(2), 805–855.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Beretti, A., Figuières, C., & Grolleau, G. (2011). Using money to motivate both ‘Saints’ and ‘Sinners’: A field experiment on motivational crowding-out. Working Paper # 2011-15. http://www.lameta.univ-montp1.fr/Documents/DR2011-15.pdf.
  7. Bolle, F. (1990). High reward experiments without high expenditure for the experimenter. Journal of Economic Psychology, 11(2), 157–167.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Bolton, G. E., Zwick, R., & Katok, E. (1998). Dictator game giving: Rules of fairness versus acts of kindness. International Journal of Game Theory, 27(2), 269–299.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Bowles, S. (2008). Policies designed for self-interested citizens may undermine ‘the moral sentiments’: Evidence from economic experiments. Science, 320(5883), 1605–1609.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Brehm, S. S., & Brehm, J. W. (1981). Psychological reactance: A theory of freedom and control. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  11. Brekke, K. A., Kverndokk, S., & Nyborg, K. (2003). An economic model of moral motivation. Journal of Public Economics, 87, 1967–1983.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Bruvoll, A., Halvorsen, B., & Nyborg, K. (2002). Households’ recycling efforts. Resources, Conservation and Recycling, 36(4), 337–354.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Cain, D., Loewenstein, G., & Moore, D. (2005). The dirt on coming clean: Perverse effects of disclosing conflicts of interest. Journal of Legal Studies, 34(1), 1–25.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Charness, G., & Gneezy, U. (2009). Incentives to exercise. Econometrica, 77(3), 909–931.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Chartier, D. (2002). ONG et société civile: Un inventaire à la Prévert. Écologie & Politique, 25, 101–106.Google Scholar
  16. Chiou, W. B., Wan, C. S., Wu, W. H., & Lee, K. T. (2011). A randomized experiment to examine unintended consequences of dietary supplement use among daily smokers: Taking supplements reduces self-regulation of smoking. Addiction, 106(12), 2221–2228.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Clary, E. G., & Snyder, M. (1999). The motivations to volunteer: Theoretical and practical considerations. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 8(5), 156–159.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Cordano, M., Welcomer, S. A., & Scherer, R. F. (2003). An analysis of the predictive validity of the New Ecological Paradigm scale. Journal of Environmental Education, 34(3), 22–28.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Cornelissen, G., Pandelaere, M., Warlop, L., & Dewitte, S. (2008). Positive cueing: Promoting sustainable consumer behavior by cueing common ecological behaviors as environmental. International Journal of Research in Marketing, 25(1), 46–55.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Costa, D. L., & Khan, M. E. (2010). Energy conservation “nudges” and environmentalist ideology: Evidence from a randomized residential electricity field experiment. NBER Working Paper No. 15939.Google Scholar
  21. Croson, R. (2007). Theories of commitment, altruism and reciprocity: Evidence from linear public goods games. Economic Inquiry, 45(2), 199–216.Google Scholar
  22. Dunlap, R. E., & Van Liere, K. D. (1978). The “new environmental paradigm”: A proposed measuring instrument and preliminary results. Journal of Environmental Education, 9(1), 10–19.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Dunlap, R. E., Van Liere, K. D., Mertig, A. G., & Emmet Jones, R. (2000). Measuring endorsement of the New Ecological Paradigm: A revised NEP Scale. Journal of Social Issues, 56(3), 425–442.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Engel, C. (2013). Behavioral law and economics: Empirical methods. Max Planck Institute for Research on Collective Goods, WP 2013/1.Google Scholar
  25. Exadaktylos, F., Espín, A. M., & Brañas-Garza, P. (2013). Experimental subjects are not different. Scientific Reports, 3, 1213. doi:10.1038/srep01213.
  26. Frey, B. S., & Jegen, R. (2001). Motivation crowding theory. Journal of Economic Survey, 15(5), 589–611.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Frey, B. S., & Meier, S. (2003). Are political economists selfish and indoctrinated? Evidence from a natural experiment. Economic Inquiry, 41(3), 448–462.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Frey, B. S., & Oberholzer-Gee, F. (1997). The cost of price incentives: An empirical analysis of motivation crowding-out. The American Economic Review, 87(4), 746–755.Google Scholar
  29. Gneezy, A., Imas, A., Brown, A., Nelson, L. D., & Norton, M. I. (2012). Paying to be nice: Consistency and costly prosocial behavior. Management Science, 58(1), 179–187.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Gneezy, U., Meier, S., & Rey-Biel, P. (2011). When and why incentives (don’t) work to modify behavior. Journal of Economic Perspectives, 25(4), 191–210.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Gneezy, U., & Rustichini, A. (2000). Pay enough or don’t pay at all. The Quarterly Journal of Economics, 115(3), 791–810.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Hahn, R. W. (1989). Economic prescriptions for environmental problems : How the patient followed the doctor’s orders. Journal of Economic Perspectives, 3(2), 95–114.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Hawcroft, L. J., & Milfont, T. L. (2010). The use (and abuse) of the new environmental paradigm scale over the last 30 years: A meta-analysis. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 30(2), 143–158.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Jolls, C. (2007). Behavioral law and economics. NBER working paper series, 12879, 1–45.Google Scholar
  35. Jolls, C., Sunstein, C., & Thaler, R. (1998). A behavioral approach to law and economics. Stanford Law Review, 50, 1471–1548.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Jordan, J., Mullen, E., & Murnighan, J. K. (2011). Striving for the moral self: The effects of recalling past moral actions on future moral behavior. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 37(5), 701–713.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Khan, U., & Dhar, R. (2006). Licensing effect in consumer choice. Journal of Marketing Research, 43(2), 357–365.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Khan, U., Dhar, R., & Schmidt, S. (2010). Giving consumers license to enjoy luxury. MIT Sloan Management Review. http://sloanreview.mit.edu/the-magazine/2010-spring/51310/giving-consumers-license-to-enjoy-luxury/.
  39. Mazar, N., & Zhong, C. (2010). Do green products make us better people? Psychological Science, 21(4), 494–498.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Nyborg, K. (1999). Informational aspect of environment policy deserves more attention: Comment on the paper by Frey. Journal of Consumer Policy, 22(4), 419–427.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Nyborg, K., Howarth, R. B., & Brekke, K. A. (2006). Green consumers and public policy: On socially contingent moral motivation. Resource and Energy Economics, 28(4), 351–366.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Sachdeva, S., Iliev, R., & Medin, D. (2009). Sinning saints and saintly sinners: The paradox of moral self-regulation. Psychological Science, 20(4), 523–528.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Thaler, R. H., & Sunstein, C. R. (2008). Nudge: Improving decisions about health, wealth, and happiness. Environmental and Resource Economics, 47(1), 149–150.Google Scholar
  44. Tiefenbeck, V., Staake, T., Roth, K., & Sachs, O. (2013). For better or for worse? Empirical evidence of moral licensing in a behavioural energy conservation campaign. Energy Policy, 57, 160–171.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Véleine, C. (2007). Les représentations et motivations des étudiants et des étudiantes en 1ère année d’école d’ingénieur/e ou vétérinaire. Lettre du réseau «Egalité des chances entre les femmes et les hommes dans l’enseignement supérieur agricole», 6, 1–8.Google Scholar
  46. Vollan, B. (2008). Socio-ecological explanations for crowding-out effects from economic field experiments in southern Africa. Ecological Economics, 67(4), 560–573.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Woodyard, C. (2009). Hybrid car owners drive more and get more traffic tickets. USA Today. http://content.usatoday.com/communities/driveon/post/2009/07/68494710/1#.U3oCyy8hB7M.
  48. Zhang, Y., Xu, J., Jiang, Z., & Huang, S. C. (2011). Been there, done that: The impact of effort investment on goal value and consumer motivation. Journal of Consumer Research, 38(1), 78–93.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media New York 2014

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.LAMETA, UMR 1135ENSAMMontpellier Cedex 1France
  2. 2.LAMETA, UMR 1135, ENSAMBurgundy School of Business, LESSACMontpellierFrance
  3. 3.LAMETA, UMR 1135INRAMontpellierFrance

Personalised recommendations