Advertisement

Tracing Enhances Recall and Transfer of Knowledge of the Water Cycle

  • Michael Tang
  • Paul GinnsEmail author
  • Michael J. Jacobson
INTERVENTION STUDY

Abstract

Cognitive load theory has incorporated evolutionary perspectives to consider how biologically primary knowledge (such as physical movement and pointing), acquired through evolutionary processes, might support the acquisition of biologically secondary knowledge (such as reading or writing), requiring explicit teaching. Tracing (a physical movement) against a surface with the index finger may be one form of biologically primary knowledge that can enhance learning biologically secondary knowledge. We investigated whether tracing lesson materials (about the water cycle) presented on A4 pieces of paper in an initial phase, then on an A1 poster in a subsequent phase, would affect primary school students’ reports of intrinsic versus extraneous cognitive load, as well as recall and transfer test performance. Students who traced while studying reported lower extraneous cognitive load than those who simply studied and scored higher on subsequent recall and transfer tests. Considerations for instructional designers, educators and researchers are discussed.

Keywords

Cognitive load theory Biologically primary knowledge Tracing Recall Transfer 

Notes

Compliance with Ethical Standards

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.

References

  1. Agostinho, S., Tindall-Ford, S., Ginns, P., Howard, S. J., Leahy, W., & Paas, F. (2015). Giving learning a helping hand: finger tracing of temperature graphs on an iPad. Educational Psychology Review, 27(3), 427–443.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-015-9315-5.Google Scholar
  2. Alibali, M. W. (2005). Gesture in spatial cognition: expressing, communicating, and thinking about spatial information. Spatial Cognition and Computation, 5(4), 307–331.  https://doi.org/10.1207/s15427633scc0504_2.Google Scholar
  3. Alibali, M. W., Kita, S., & Young, A. J. (2000). Gesture and the process of speech production: we think, therefore we gesture. Language and Cognitive Processes, 15(6), 593–613.  https://doi.org/10.1080/016909600750040571.Google Scholar
  4. Anderson, L. W., Krathwohl, D. R., et al. (2013). Taxonomy for learning, teaching, and assessing: a revision of Bloom’s taxonomy of educational objectives. Pearson.Google Scholar
  5. Bara, F., Gentaz, E., Colé, P., & Sprenger-Charolles, L. (2004). The visuo-haptic and haptic exploration of letters increases the kindergarten-children’s understanding of the alphabetic principle. Cognitive Development, 19(3), 433–449.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2004.05.003.Google Scholar
  6. Bara, F., Gentaz, E., & Colé, P. (2007). Haptics in learning to read with children from low socio-economic status families. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 25(4), 643–663.  https://doi.org/10.1348/026151007X186643.Google Scholar
  7. Barsalou, L. W. (1999). Perceptual symbol systems. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22(04), 577–660.  https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X99002149.Google Scholar
  8. Bates, E., Camaioni, L., & Volterra, V. (1975). The acquisition of performatives prior to speech. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly of Behavior and Development, 21, 205–226.Google Scholar
  9. Beilock, S. L., & Holt, L. E. (2007). Embodied preference judgments: can likeability be driven by the motor system? Psychological Science, 18(1), 51–57.  https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01848.x.Google Scholar
  10. Board of Studies New South Wales (BOS NSW). (2006). Science K-10 syllabus. Sydney: Board of Studies NSW.Google Scholar
  11. Boucheix, J. M., & Lowe, R. K. (2010). An eye tracking comparison of external pointing cues and internal continuous cues in learning with complex animations. Learning and Instruction, 20(2), 123–135.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.learninstruc.2009.02.015.Google Scholar
  12. Britton, B. K., Glynn, S. M., Meyer, B. J., & Penland, M. J. (1982). Effects of text structure on use of cognitive capacity during reading. Journal of Educational Psychology, 74(1), 51–61.Google Scholar
  13. Bunn, T. (1996). Form 6 biology revision. Auckland: ESA Publications.Google Scholar
  14. Camaioni, L. (1993). The development of intentional communication: a re-analysis. In J. Nadel & L. Camaioni (Eds.), New perspectives in early communicative development (pp. 82–96). New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
  15. Cohen, J. (1968). Weighted kappa: nominal scale agreement provision for scaled disagreement or partial credit. Psychological Bulletin, 70(4), 213–220.Google Scholar
  16. Colonnesi, C., Stams, G. J. J., Koster, I., & Noom, M. J. (2010). The relation between pointing and language development: a meta-analysis. Developmental Review, 30(4), 352–366.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2010.10.001.Google Scholar
  17. Cosman, J. D., & Vecera, S. P. (2010). Attention affects visual perceptual processing near the hand. Psychological Science, 21(9), 1254–1258.  https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797610380697.Google Scholar
  18. Crutzen, R., & Peters, G. J. Y. (2017). Scale quality: alpha is an inadequate estimate and factor-analytic evidence is needed first of all. Health Psychology Review, 11(3), 242–247.Google Scholar
  19. Cumming, G. (2012). Understanding the new statistics: effect sizes, confidence intervals, and meta-analysis. New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
  20. Davoli, C. C., & Brockmole, J. R. (2012). The hands shield attention from visual interference. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 74(7), 1386–1390.  https://doi.org/10.3758/s13414-012-0351-7.Google Scholar
  21. de Koning, B. B., Tabbers, H. K., Rikers, R. M., & Paas, F. (2007). Attention cueing as a means to enhance learning from an animation. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 21(6), 731–746.  https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.1346.Google Scholar
  22. de Koning, B. B., Tabbers, H. K., Rikers, R. M., & Paas, F. (2009). Towards a framework for attention cueing in instructional animations: guidelines for research and design. Educational Psychology Review, 21(2), 113–140.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-009-9098-7.Google Scholar
  23. de Koning, B. B., Tabbers, H. K., Rikers, R. M., & Paas, F. (2010). Attention guidance in learning from a complex animation: seeing is understanding? Learning and Instruction, 20(2), 111–122.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.learninstruc.2009.02.010.Google Scholar
  24. de Villiers Rader, N., & Zukow-Goldring, P. (2010). How the hands control attention during early word learning. Gesture, 10(2-3), 202–221.  https://doi.org/10.1075/gest.10.2-3.05rad.Google Scholar
  25. Dodd, M. D., & Shumborski, S. (2009). Examining the influence of action on spatial working memory: the importance of selection. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 62(6), 1236–1247.  https://doi.org/10.1080/17470210802439869.Google Scholar
  26. Foglia, L., & Wilson, R. A. (2013). Embodied cognition. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 4(3), 319–325.  https://doi.org/10.1002/wcs.1226.Google Scholar
  27. Geary, D. C. (2008). An evolutionarily informed education science. Educational Psychologist, 43(4), 179–195.  https://doi.org/10.1080/00461520802392133.Google Scholar
  28. Ginns, P. (2006) Integrating information: a meta-analysis of the spatial contiguity and temporal contiguity effects. Learning and Instruction 16(6):511–525.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.learninstruc.2006.10.001.
  29. Ginns, P., Hu, F. T., Byrne, E., & Bobis, J. (2016). Learning by tracing worked examples. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 30(2), 160–169.  https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.3171.Google Scholar
  30. Glenberg, A. M. (1997). What memory is for. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 20, 1–55.Google Scholar
  31. Gordon, C. (2012). Self-management of students’ cognitive load within a primary school context (unpublished Honours thesis). University of Wollongong, New South Wales, Australia.Google Scholar
  32. Hattie, J. A. (2009). Visible learning: a synthesis of 800+ meta-analyses on achievement. Abingdon. Routledge.Google Scholar
  33. Hayes, A. F. (2017). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis: a regression-based approach (2nd ed.). New York, NY: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  34. Hostetter, A. B. (2011). When do gestures communicate? A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 137(2), 297–315.  https://doi.org/10.1037/a0022128.Google Scholar
  35. Houston, S. D. (2004). The first writing: script invention as history and process. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  36. Hu, F. T., Ginns, P., & Bobis, J. (2014). Does tracing worked examples enhance geometry learning? Australian Journal of Educational & Developmental Psychology, 14, 45–49.Google Scholar
  37. Hu, F. T., Ginns, P., & Bobis, J. (2015). Getting the point: tracing worked examples enhances learning. Learning and Instruction, 35, 85–93.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.learninstruc.2014.10.002.Google Scholar
  38. Hulme, C. (1979). The interaction of visual and motor memory for graphic forms following tracing. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 31(2), 249–261.  https://doi.org/10.1080/14640747908400724.Google Scholar
  39. Hulme, C., Monk, A., & Ives, S. (1987). Some experimental studies of multi-sensory teaching: the effects of manual tracing on children’s paired-associate learning. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 5(4), 299–307.Google Scholar
  40. Imhof, B., Scheiter, K., Edelmann, J., & Gerjets, P. (2013). Learning about locomotion patterns: effective use of multiple pictures and motion-indicating arrows. Computers & Education, 65, 45–55.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.compedu.2013.01.017.Google Scholar
  41. Johnson, C. I., & Mayer, R. E. (2012). An eye movement analysis of the spatial contiguity effect in multimedia learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Applied, 18(2), 178–191.  https://doi.org/10.1037/a0026923.Google Scholar
  42. Justice, L. M., & Ezell, H. K. (2004). Print referencing: an emergent literacy enhancement strategy and its clinical applications. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 35(2), 185–193.  https://doi.org/10.1044/0161-1461.Google Scholar
  43. Justice, L. M., Kaderavek, J. N., Fan, X., Sofka, A., & Hunt, A. (2009). Accelerating preschoolers’ early literacy development through classroom-based teacher–child storybook reading and explicit print referencing. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 40(1), 67–85.  https://doi.org/10.1044/0161-1461(2008/07-0098).Google Scholar
  44. Kalenine, S., Pinet, L., & Gentaz, E. (2011). The visual and visuo-haptic exploration of geometrical shapes increases their recognition in preschoolers. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 35(1), 18–26.  https://doi.org/10.1177/0165025410367443.Google Scholar
  45. Kalyuga, S. (2007). Expertise reversal effect and its implications for learner-tailored instruction. Educational Psychology Review, 19(4), 509–539.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-007-9054-3.Google Scholar
  46. Kalyuga, S. (2011). Cognitive load theory: how many types of load does it really need? Educational Psychology Review, 23(1), 1–19.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-010-9150-7.Google Scholar
  47. Keselman, H. J., Othman, A.R., & Wilcox, R.R. (2013). Preliminary testing for normality: is this a good practice? Journal of Modern Applied Statistical Methods, 12, Article 2. Retrieved from http://digitalcommons.wayne.edu/jmasm/vol12/iss2/2
  48. Kirschner, P. A., Sweller, J., Kirschner, F., & Zambrano, J. (2018). From cognitive load theory to collaborative cognitive load theory. International Journal of Computer-Supported Collaborative Learning, 13(2), 213–233.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s11412-018-9277-y.Google Scholar
  49. Leppink, J., Paas, F., van der Vleuten, C. P., van Gog, T., & van Merriënboer, J. J. (2013). Development of an instrument for measuring different types of cognitive load. Behavior Research Methods, 45(4), 1058–1072.  https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-013-0334-1.Google Scholar
  50. Liszkowski, U., Brown, P., Callaghan, T., Takada, A., & De Vos, C. (2012). A prelinguistic gestural universal of human communication. Cognitive Science, 36(4), 698–713.  https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1551-6709.2011.01228.x.Google Scholar
  51. Loman, N. L., & Mayer, R. E. (1983). Signaling techniques that increase the understandability of expository prose. Journal of Educational Psychology, 75(3), 402–412.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.75.3.402.Google Scholar
  52. Macken, L., & Ginns, P. (2014). Pointing and tracing gestures may enhance anatomy and physiology learning. Medical Teacher, 36(7), 596–601.  https://doi.org/10.3109/0142159X.2014.899684.Google Scholar
  53. Marshall, S. P. (1995). Schemas in problem solving. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  54. Martin, A. J., & Evans, P. (2018). Load reduction instruction: exploring a framework that assesses explicit instruction through to independent learning. Teaching and Teacher Education, 73, 203–214.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tate.2018.03.018.Google Scholar
  55. McDonald, R. P. (1999). Test theory: a unified treatment. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
  56. McNeill, D. (1992). Hand and mind: what gesture reveals about thought. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
  57. Miller, G. A. (1956). The magical number seven, plus or minus two: some limits on our capacity for processing information. Psychological Review, 63(2), 81–97.  https://doi.org/10.1037/h0043158.Google Scholar
  58. Montessori, M. (1912). The Montessori method. London: William Heinemann.Google Scholar
  59. Montessori, M. (1914). Dr. Montessori’s own handbook. London: William Heinemann.Google Scholar
  60. Newcombe, R. G. (2006a). Confidence intervals for an effect size measure based on the Mann–Whitney statistic. Part 1: general issues and tail-area-based methods. Statistics in Medicine, 25(4), 543–557.  https://doi.org/10.1002/sim.2323.Google Scholar
  61. Newcombe, R. G. (2006b). Confidence intervals for an effect size measure based on the Mann–Whitney statistic. Part 2: asymptotic methods and evaluation. Statistics in Medicine, 25(4), 559–573.  https://doi.org/10.1002/sim.2324.Google Scholar
  62. Nowicki, S. (2012). Holt McDougal biology: Interactive reader. Houghton Mifflin Harcourt.Google Scholar
  63. Paas, F., & Sweller, J. (2012). An evolutionary upgrade of cognitive load theory: using the human motor system and collaboration to support the learning of complex cognitive tasks. Educational Psychology Review, 24(1), 27–45.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-011-9179-2.Google Scholar
  64. Pefani, N. (2005). Excel Science Study Guide Years 7–8. NSW: Pascal Press.Google Scholar
  65. Peterson, L., & Peterson, M. J. (1959). Short-term retention of individual verbal items. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 58(3), 193–198.  https://doi.org/10.1037/h0049234.Google Scholar
  66. Ping, R., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2010). Gesturing saves cognitive resources when talking about nonpresent objects. Cognitive Science, 34(4), 602–619.  https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1551-6709.2010.01102.x.Google Scholar
  67. Pollock, E., Chandler, P., & Sweller, J. (2002). Assimilating complex information. Learning and Instruction, 12(1), 61–86.  https://doi.org/10.1016/S0959-4752(01)00016-0.Google Scholar
  68. Pouw, W. T., Mavilidi, M. F., van Gog, T., & Paas, F. (2016). Gesturing during mental problem solving reduces eye movements, especially for individuals with lower visual working memory capacity. Cognitive Processing, 17(3), 269–277.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s10339-016-0757-6.Google Scholar
  69. Povinelli, D. J., Reaux, J. E., Bierschwale, D. T., Allain, A. D., & Simon, B. B. (1997). Exploitation of pointing as a referential gesture in young children, but not adolescent chimpanzees. Cognitive Development, 12(4), 423–461.  https://doi.org/10.1016/S0885-2014(97)90017-4.Google Scholar
  70. Reed, C. L., Grubb, J. D., & Steele, C. (2006). Hands up: attentional prioritization of space near the hand. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 32(1), 166–177.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.32.1.166.Google Scholar
  71. Sharpe, K., & Van Gelder, L. (2006). Evidence for cave marking by Paleolithic children. Antiquity, 80(310), 937–947.Google Scholar
  72. Sheng, Y., & Sheng, Z. (2012). Is coefficient alpha robust to non-normal data? Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 1–13.  https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00034.Google Scholar
  73. Steinbach, M. J., & Held, R. (1968). Eye tracking of observer-generated target movements. Science, 161(3837), 187–188.Google Scholar
  74. Sweller, J. (2008). Instructional implications of David C. Geary’s evolutionary educational psychology. Educational Psychologist, 43(4), 214–216.  https://doi.org/10.1080/00461520802392208.
  75. Sweller, J. (2010). Element interactivity and intrinsic, extraneous, and germane cognitive load. Educational Psychology Review, 22(2), 123–138.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-010-9128-5.Google Scholar
  76. Sweller, J. (2011). Cognitive load theory. In J. P. Mestre & B. H. Ross (Eds.), Psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 55, pp. 37–76). Academic Press.Google Scholar
  77. Sweller, J., van Merriënboer, J. J., & Paas, F. G. (1998). Cognitive architecture and instructional design. Educational Psychology Review, 10(3), 251–296.  https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1022193728205.Google Scholar
  78. Sweller, J., Ayres, P., & Kalyuga, S. (2011). Cognitive load theory. New York, NY: Springer.Google Scholar
  79. Tomasello, M., Carpenter, M., & Liszkowski, U. (2007). A new look at infant pointing. Child Development, 78(3), 705–722.  https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01025.x.Google Scholar
  80. Tseng, P., & Bridgeman, B. (2011). Improved change detection with nearby hands. Experimental Brain Research, 209(1), 257–269.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.scient.2012.11.006.Google Scholar
  81. Van Gelder, L. (2015). Counting the children: the role of children in the production of finger flutings in four Upper Palaeolithic caves. Oxford Journal of Archaeology, 34(2), 119–138.  https://doi.org/10.1111/ojoa.12052.Google Scholar
  82. Vygotsky, L. S. (2004). Mastering attention. In R. W. Rieber & D. K. Robinson (Eds.), The essential Vygotsky (pp. 375–400). Boston, MA: Springer.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC, part of Springer Nature 2019

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Sydney School of Education and Social WorkThe University of SydneySydneyAustralia

Personalised recommendations