Advertisement

Educational Psychology Review

, Volume 28, Issue 4, pp 831–852 | Cite as

Rethinking the Boundaries of Cognitive Load Theory in Complex Learning

  • Slava Kalyuga
  • Anne-Marie Singh
Reflection on the Field

Abstract

In the traditional framework of cognitive load theory, it is assumed that the acquisition of domain-specific knowledge structures (or schemas) is the only instructional goal, and therefore, the theory is applicable to any instructional task. Accordingly, the basic concepts of intrinsic (productive) and extraneous (unproductive) types of cognitive load were defined based on the relevance (or irrelevance) of the corresponding cognitive processes that impose the load to achieving this universal instructional goal, and the instructional methods advocated by this theory are aimed at enhancing the acquisition of domain-specific schemas. The paper suggests considering this goal within the whole variety of possible specific goals of different learner activities that could be involved in complex learning. This would result in narrowing down of boundaries of cognitive load theory and have implications for distinguishing types of cognitive load, sequencing different goals and instructional tasks, considering the role of learner expertise, and other aspects of complex learning. One of the consequences of this reconceptualization is abandoning the rigid explicit instruction versus minimal guidance dichotomy and replacing it with a more flexible approach based on differentiating specific goals of various learner activities in complex learning. In particular, it may allow reconciling seemingly contradictory results from studies of the effectiveness of worked examples in cognitive load theory (supporting the initial fully guided explicit instruction for novice learners) and studies within the frameworks of productive failure and invention learning that have reportedly demonstrated that minimally guided tasks provided prior to explicit instruction might benefit novice learners.

Keywords

Cognitive load theory Instructional guidance Instructional goals Explicit instruction Initial problem solving Expertise reversal effect 

References

  1. Alfieri, L., Brooks, P. J., Aldrich, N. J., & Tenenbaum, H. R. (2011). Does discovery-based instruction enhance learning? Journal of Educational Psychology, 103, 1–18.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Atkinson, R. K., Derry, S. J., Renkl, A., & Wortham, D. (2000). Learning from examples: instructional principles from the worked example research. Review of Educational Research, 70, 181–214.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Baddeley, A. (1986). Working memory. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  4. Barrouillet, P., Bernardin, S., & Camos, V. (2004). Time constraints and resource sharing in adults’ working memory spans. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 133, 83–100.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Barrouillet, P., Bernardin, S., Portrat, S., Vergauwe, E., & Camos, V. (2007). Time and cognitive load in working memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 33(3), 570–585.Google Scholar
  6. Bransford, J. D., & Schwartz, D. L. (1999). Rethinking transfer: a simple proposal with multiple implications. Review of Research in Education, 24(3), 61–100.Google Scholar
  7. Capon, N., & Kuhn, D. (2004). What’s so good about problem-based learning? Cognition and Instruction, 22(1), 61–79.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Cooper, G., & Sweller, J. (1987). Effects of schema acquisition and rule automation on mathematical problem-solving transfer. Journal of Educational Psychology, 79, 347–362.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Cowan, N. (2001). The magical number 4 in short-term memory: a reconsideration of mental storage capacity. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24, 87–185.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. De Grave, W. S., Schmidt, H. G., & Boshuizen, H. P. A. (2001). Effects of problem-based discussion on studying a subsequent text: a randomized trial among first year medical students. Instructional Science, 29(1), 33–44.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. De Jong, T. (2005). The guided discovery principle in multimedia learning. In R. E. Mayer (Ed.), The Cambridge handbook of multimedia learning (pp. 215–228). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. De Jong, T. (2010). Cognitive load theory, educational research, and instructional design: some food for thought. Instructional Science, 38, 105–134.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Diamond, A. (2013). Executive functions. Annual Review of Psychology, 64, 135–168.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Ericsson, K. A., & Kintsch, W. (1995). Long-term working memory. Psychological Review, 102, 211–245.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Geary, D. C. (2007). Educating the evolved mind: conceptual foundations for an evolutionary educational psychology. In J. S. Carlson & J. R. Levin (Eds.), Psychological perspectives on contemporary educational issues (pp. 1–99). Greenwich, CT: Information Age Publishing.Google Scholar
  16. Gerjets, P., & Scheiter, K. (2003). Goal configurations and processing strategies as moderators between instructional design and cognitive load: evidence from hypertext-based instruction. Educational Psychologist, 38, 33–42.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Glogger-Frey, I., Fleischer, C., Grüny, L., Kappich, J., & Renkl, A. (2015). Inventing a solution and studying a worked solution prepare differently for learning from direct instruction. Learning and Instruction, 39, 72–87.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Hmelo-Silver, C. E. (2004). Problem-based learning: what and how do students learn? Educational Psychology Review, 16(3), 235–266.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Hmelo-Silver, C. E., Duncan, R. G., & Chinn, C. A. (2007). Scaffolding and achievement in problem-based and inquiry learning: a response to Kirschner, Sweller, and Clark (2006). Educational Psychologist, 42(2), 99–107.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Hsu, C.-Y., Kalyuga, S., & Sweller, J. (2015). When should guidance be presented during physics instruction? Archives of Scientific Psychology, 3, 37–53.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Kalyuga, S. (2006). Assessment of learners’ organised knowledge structures in adaptive learning environments. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 20, 333–342.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Kalyuga, S. (2007). Expertise reversal effect and its implications for learner-tailored instruction. Educational Psychology Review, 19, 509–539.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Kalyuga, S. (2011). Cognitive load theory: how many types of load does it really need? Educational Psychology Review, 23, 1–19.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Kalyuga, S. (2015). Instructional guidance: a cognitive load perspective. Charlotte, NC: Information Age Publishing.Google Scholar
  25. Kalyuga, S., Ayres, P., Chandler, P., & Sweller, J. (2003). The expertise reversal effect. Educational Psychologist, 38, 23–31.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Kalyuga, S., Renkl, A., & Paas, F. (2010). Facilitating flexible problem solving: a cognitive load perspective. Educational Psychology Review, 22, 175–186.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Kapur, M. (2008). Productive failure. Cognition and Instruction, 26, 379–424.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Kapur, M. (2010). Productive failure in mathematical problem solving. Instructional Science, 38(6), 523–550.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Kapur, M. (2011). A further study of productive failure in mathematical problem solving: unpacking the design components. Instructional Science, 39, 561–579.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Kapur, M. (2012). Productive failure in learning the concept of variance. Instructional Science, 40(4), 651–672.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Kapur, M. (2014). Productive failure in learning math. Cognitive Science, 38, 1008–1022.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Kapur, M., & Bielaczyc, K. (2012). Designing for productive failure. The Journal of the Learning Sciences, 21(1), 45–83.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Kapur, M., & Rummel, N. (2012). Productive failure in learning from generation and invention activities. Instructional Science, 40, 645–650.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Kirschner, P. A., Sweller, J., & Clark, R. E. (2006). Why minimal guidance during instruction does not work: an analysis of the failure of constructivist, discovery, problem-based, experiential, and inquiry-based teaching. Educational Psychologist, 41, 75–86.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Klahr, D. (2009). To every thing there is a season, and a time to every purpose under the heavens: what about direct instruction? In S. Tobias & T. M. Duffy (Eds.), Constructivist instruction: success or failure? (pp. 291–310). New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
  36. Koedinger, K. R., & McLaughlin, E. A. (2014). The knowledge-learning-instruction (KLI) dependency: how the domain-specific and domain-general interact in STEM learning. In M. McDaniel, R. Frey, S. Fitzpatrick, & H. L. Roediger (Eds.), Integrating cognitive science with innovative teaching in STEM disciplines (pp. 53–73). St. Louis, Missouri: Washington University Libraries.Google Scholar
  37. Koedinger, K. R., Corbett, A. T., & Perfetti, C. (2012). The knowledge-learning-instruction (KLI) framework: bridging the science-practice chasm to enhance robust student learning. Cognitive Science, 36, 757–798.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Kuhn, D. (2007). Is direct instruction an answer to the right question? Educational Psychologist, 42, 109–113.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Lee, H. S., & Anderson, J. R. (2013). Student learning: what has instruction got to do with it? Annual Review of Psychology, 64, 3.1–3.25.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Loibl, K., & Rummel, N. (2014). Knowing what you don't know makes failure productive. Learning and Instruction, 34, 74–85.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Lorch, R. F., Jr., Lorch, E. P., Calderhead, W. J., Dunlap, E. E., Hodell, E. C., & Freer, B. D. (2010). Learning the control of variables strategy in higher and lower achieving classrooms: contributions of explicit instruction and experimentation. Journal of Educational Psychology, 102, 90–101.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Mayer, R. E. (2004). Should there be a three-strikes rule against pure discovery learning? American Psychologist, 59, 14–19.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Miyake, A., & Shah, P. (Eds.). (1999). Models of working memory: mechanisms of active maintenance and executive control. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  44. Peterson, L., & Peterson, M. J. (1959). Short-term retention of individual verbal items. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 58, 193–198.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Reisslein, J., Atkinson, R. K., Seeling, P., & Reisslein, M. (2006). Encountering the expertise reversal effect with a computer-based environment on electrical circuit analysis. Learning and Instruction, 16, 92–103.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Renkl, A. (1997). Learning from worked-out examples: a study on individual differences. Cognitive Science, 21, 1–29.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Renkl, A. (2014). Towards an instructionally-oriented theory of example-based learning. Cognitive Science, 38, 1–37.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Renkl, A., & Atkinson, R. K. (2003). Structuring the transition from example study to problem solving in cognitive skills acquisition: a cognitive load perspective. Educational Psychologist, 38, 15–22.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Roll, I., Holmes, N. G., Day, J., & Bonn, D. (2012). Evaluating metacognitive scaffolding in guided invention activities. Instructional Science, 40, 691–710.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Salden, R. J. C. M., Paas, F., & van Merriënboer, J. J. G. (2006). Personalised adaptive task selection in air traffic control: effects on training efficiency and transfer. Learning and Instruction, 16, 350–362.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Salden, R. J. C. M., Aleven, V., Schwonke, R., & Renkl, A. (2010). The expertise reversal effect and worked examples in tutored problem solving. Instructional Science, 38, 289–307.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Schmidt, R. A., & Bjork, R. A. (1992). New conceptualizations of practice: common principles in three paradigms suggest new concepts for training. Psychological Science, 3, 207–217.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Schmidt, H. G., Loyens, S. M. M., Van Gog, T., & Paas, F. (2007). Problem-based learning is compatible with human cognitive architecture: commentary on Kirschner, Sweller, and Clark (2006). Educational Psychologist, 42, 91–97.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Schmidt, H. G., van der Molen, H. T., te Winkel, W. W. R., & Wijnen, W. H. F. W. (2009). Constructivist, problem-based learning does work: a meta-analysis of curricular comparisons involving a single medical school. Educational Psychologist, 44(4), 227–249.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Schnotz, W., & Kürschner, C. (2007). A reconsideration of cognitive load theory. Educational Psychology Review, 19, 469–508.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. Schwartz, D., & Bransford, J. D. (1998). A time for telling. Cognition and Instruction, 16, 475–522.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  57. Schwartz, D. L., & Martin, T. (2004). Inventing to prepare for future learning: the hidden efficiency of encouraging original student production in statistics instruction. Cognition and Instruction, 22, 129–84.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  58. Schwartz, D., Lindgren, R., & Lewis, S. (2009). Constructivist in an age of non-constructivist assessments. In S. Tobias & T. Duffy (Eds.), Constructivist instruction: success of failure? (pp. 34–61). New York, NY: Routledge.Google Scholar
  59. Schwartz, D. L., Chase, C. C., Oppezzo, M. A., & Chin, D. B. (2011). Practicing versus inventing with contrasting cases: the effects of telling first on learning and transfer. Journal of Educational Psychology, 103, 759–75.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  60. Scott, B. M., & Schwartz, N. H. (2007). Navigational spatial displays: the role of metacognition as cognitive load. Learning and Instruction, 17, 89–105.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  61. Sweller, J. (1988). Cognitive load during problem solving: effects on learning. Cognitive Science, 12, 257–285.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  62. Sweller, J. (1994). Cognitive load theory, learning difficulty, and instructional design. Learning and Instruction, 4, 295–312.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  63. Sweller, J. (2003). Evolution of human cognitive architecture. In B. Ross (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 43, pp. 215–266). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  64. Sweller, J. (2010). Element interactivity and intrinsic, extraneous, and germane cognitive load. Educational Psychology Review, 22, 123–138.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  65. Sweller, J., Kirschner, P. A., & Clark, R. E. (2007). Why minimally guided teaching techniques do not work: a reply to commentaries. Educational Psychologist, 42(2), 115–121.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  66. Sweller, J., Ayres, P., & Kalyuga, S. (2011). Cognitive load theory. New York: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  67. Van Gog, T., Kester, L., & Paas, F. (2011). Effects of worked examples, example-problem, and problem-example pairs on novices’ learning. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 36, 212–218.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  68. Van Merriënboer, J. J. G. (1990). Strategies for programming instruction in high school: program completion vs. program generation. Journal of Educational Computing Research, 6, 265–287.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  69. Van Merriënboer, J. J. G., & Kirschner, P. A. (2007). Ten steps to complex learning. New York: Taylor & Francis.Google Scholar
  70. Van Merrienboer, J. J. G., Kirschner, P. A., & Kester, L. (2003). Taking the load off a learner's mind: instructional design principles for complex learning. Educational Psychologist, 38, 5–13.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  71. White, B. (1993). ThinkerTools: causal models, conceptual change, and science education. Cognition and Instruction, 10(1), 1–100.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  72. White, B. Y., & Frederiksen, J. R. (1998). Inquiry, modeling, and metacognition: making science accessible to all students. Cognition and Instruction, 16(1), 3–118.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  73. Wijnia, L., Loyens, S. M. M., van Gog, T., Derous, E., & Schmidt, H. G. (2014). Is there a role for direct instruction in problem-based learning? Comparing student-constructed versus integrated model answers. Learning and Instruction, 34, 22–31.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media New York 2015

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.School of EducationUniversity of New South WalesSydneyAustralia

Personalised recommendations