Advertisement

Educational Psychology Review

, Volume 27, Issue 4, pp 645–656 | Cite as

When Is It Better to Learn Together? Insights from Research on Collaborative Learning

  • Timothy J. Nokes-MalachEmail author
  • J. Elizabeth Richey
  • Soniya Gadgil
Review Article

Abstract

Although collaboration is often considered a beneficial learning strategy, research examining the claim suggests a much more complex picture. Critically, the question is not whether collaboration is beneficial to learning, but instead how and when collaboration improves outcomes. In this paper, we first discuss the mechanisms hypothesized to support and hinder group learning. We then review insights and illustrative findings from research in cognitive, social, and educational psychology. We conclude by proposing areas for future research to expand theories of collaboration while identifying important features for educators to consider when deciding when and how to include collaboration in instructional activities.

Keywords

Collaboration Learning Mechanisms Instruction 

Notes

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by Grant SBE0836012 from the National Science Foundation, Pittsburgh Science of Learning Center (http://www.learnlab.org). No endorsement should be inferred. We thank members of Cognitive Science Learning Laboratory, Andrew Butler, Shana Carpenter, and two anonymous reviewers for their many helpful comments and suggestions on the paper.

References

  1. Andersson, J., & Rönnberg, J. (1995). Recall suffers from collaboration: joint recall effects of friendship and task complexity. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 9(3), 199–211.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Aronson, E., Blaney, N., Stephan, C., Sikes, J., & Snapp, M. (1978). The jigsaw classroom. Beverly Hills: Sage.Google Scholar
  3. Arthur, W., Jr., Day, E. A., Bennett, W., Jr., McNelly, T. L., & Jordan, J. A. (1997). Dyadic versus individual training protocols: loss and reacquisition of a complex skill. Journal of Applied Psychology, 82(5), 783–791.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Azmitia, M. (1988). Peer interaction and problem solving: when are two heads better than one? Child Development, 59(1), 87–96.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Bandura, A. (1986). Social foundations of thought and action: A social cognitive theory. Englewood Cliffs: Prentice Hall.Google Scholar
  6. Barber, S. J., Rajaram, S., & Fox, E. B. (2012). Learning and remembering with others: the key role of retrieval in shaping group recall and collective memory. Social Cognition, 30(1), 121–122.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Barron, B. (2003). When smart groups fail. The Journal of the Learning Sciences, 12(3), 307–359.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Basden, B. H., Basden, D. R., Bryner, S., & Thomas, R. L. (1997). A comparison of group and individual remembering: does collaboration disrupt retrieval strategies? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 23(5), 1176–1191.Google Scholar
  9. Basden, B. H., Basden, D. R., & Henry, S. (2000). Costs and benefits of collaborative remembering. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 14, 497–507.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Beers, P. J., Boshuizen, H. P. A., Kirschner, P. A., & Gijselaers, W. H. (2005). Computer support for knowledge construction in collaborative learning environments. Computers in Human Behavior, 21, 623–643.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Bloodgood, J. W. (2002). Quintilian: a classic educator speaks to the writing process. Reading, Research and Instruction, 42(1), 30–43.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Bossert, S. T. (1988/1989). Cooperative activities in the classroom. Review of Research in Education, 15, 225–250.Google Scholar
  13. Chi, M. T. H. (2009). Active-constructive-interactive: a conceptual framework for differentiating learning activities. Topics in Cognitive Science, 1(1), 73–105.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Chi, M. T. H., & Wiley, R. (2014). The ICAP framework: linking cognitive engagement to active learning outcomes. Educational Psychologist, 49(4), 219–243.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Collaros, P. A., & Anderson, L. R. (1969). Effect of perceived expertness upon creativity of members of brainstorming groups. Journal of Applied Psychology, 53(2), 159–163.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Congleton, A. R., & Rajaram, S. (2011). The influence of learning methods on collaboration: prior repeated retrieval enhances retrieval organization, abolishes collaborative inhibition, and promotes post-collaborative memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 140(4), 535–551.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Craig, S. D., Chi, M. T. H., & VanLehn, K. (2009). Improving classroom learning by collaboratively observing human tutoring videos while problem solving. Journal of Educational Psychology, 101(4), 779–789.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Crooks, S. M., Klein, J. D., Savenye, W., & Leader, L. (1998). Effects of cooperative and individual learning during learner-controlled computer-based instruction. The Journal of Experimental Education, 66(3), 223–244.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Diehl, M., & Stroebe, W. (1987). Productivity loss in brainstorming groups: toward the solution of a riddle. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53, 497–509.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Dillenbourg, P. (1999). What do you mean by collaborative learning? In P. Dillenbourg (Ed.), Collaborative-learning: Cognitive and computational approaches (pp. 1–19). Oxford: Elsevier.Google Scholar
  21. Dillenbourg, P., Baker, M., Blaye, A., & O’Malley, C. (1996). The evolution of research on collaborative learning. In E. Spada & P. Reiman (Eds.), Learning in humans and machine: Towards an interdisciplinary learning science (pp. 189–211). Oxford: Elsevier.Google Scholar
  22. Engle, R. A. (2012). The productive disciplinary engagement framework: Origins, key concepts, and developments. In D. Y. Dai (Ed.), Design research on learning and thinking in educational settings (pp. 161–200). New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
  23. Finlay, F., Hitch, G. J., & Meudell, P. R. (2000). Mutual inhibition in collaborative recall: evidence for a retrieval-based account. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 26(6), 1556–1567.Google Scholar
  24. Gadgil, S., & Nokes-Malach, T. J. (2012). Collaborative facilitation through error-detection: a classroom experiment. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 26(3), 410–420.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Gholson, B., & Craig, S. D. (2006). Promoting constructive activities that support vicarious learning during computer-based instruction. Educational Psychology Review, 18, 119–139.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Harris, C. B., Keil, P. G., Sutton, J., Barnier, A. J., & McIlwain, D. J. F. (2011). We remember, we forget: collaborative remembering in older couples. Discourse Processes, 48, 267–303.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Harris, C. B., Barnier, A. J., & Sutton, J. (2013). Shared encoding and the costs and benefits of collaborative recall. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 39(1), 183–195.Google Scholar
  28. Hill, G. W. (1982). Group versus individual performance: are n + 1 heads better than one. Psychological Bulletin, 91(3), 517–539.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Johansson, N. O., Andersson, J., & Rönnberg, J. (2005). Compensating strategies in collaborative remembering in very old couples. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 46, 349–359.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Johnson, D., & Johnson, R. (1985). The internal dynamics of cooperative learning groups. In R. Slavin, S. Sharan, S. Kagan, R. Hertz-Lazarowitz, C. Webb, & R. Schmuck (Eds.), Learning to cooperate, cooperating to learn (pp. 103–124). New York: Plenum Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Johnson, D. W., & Johnson, R. T. (1989). Cooperation and competition: Theory and research. Edina: Interaction Book Company.Google Scholar
  32. Johnson, D. W., & Johnson, R. T. (2009). An educational psychology success story: social interdependence theory and cooperative learning. Educational Researcher, 38(5), 365–379.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Johnson, D. W., Johnson, R. T., & Stanne, M. B. (2000). Cooperative learning methods: a meta-analysis. Retrieved April 14, 2015 from www.tablelearning.com/uploads/File/EXHIBIT-B.pdf.
  34. Johnson, D. W., Johnson, R. T., & Smith, K. (2007). The state of cooperative learning in postsecondary and professional settings. Educational Psychology Review, 19(1), 15–29.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Karau, S. J., & Williams, K. D. (1993). Social loafing: a meta-analytic review and theoretical integration. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65(4), 681–706.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Karpicke, J. D., & Blunt, J. R. (2011). Retrieval practice produces more learning than elaborative studying with concept mapping. Science, 331, 772–775.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Kelley, M. R., & Wright, D. B. (2010). Obtaining representative nominal groups. Behavior Research Methods, 42(1), 36–41.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Kerr, N. L., & Tindale, R. S. (2004). Group performance and decision making. Annual Review of Psychology, 55(1), 623–655.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Kirschner, F., Paas, F., & Kirschner, P. A. (2009a). A cognitive load approach to collaborative learning: united brains for complex tasks. Educational Psychology Review, 21(1), 31–42.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Kirschner, F., Paas, F., & Kirschner, P. A. (2009b). Individual and group based learning from complex cognitive tasks: effects on retention and transfer efficiency. Computers in Human Behavior, 25(2), 306–314.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Kirschner, F., Paas, F., & Kirschner, P. A. (2011). Task complexity as a driver for collaborative learning efficiency: the collective working-memory effect. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 25(4), 615–624.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Kollias, V., Mamalougos, N., Vamvakoussi, X., Lakkala, M., & Vosniadou, S. (2005). Teachers’ attitudes to and beliefs about web-based collaborative learning environments in the context of an international implementation. Computers in Education, 45(3), 295–315.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Kuhn, D., & Crowell, A. (2011). Dialogic argumentation as a vehicle for developing young adolescents? Thinking. Psychological Science, 22, 545–552.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Latane, B., Williams, K., & Harkins, S. (1979). Many hands make light work: the causes and consequences of social loafing. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 822–832.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Laughlin, P. R., Hatch, E. C., Silver, J. S., & Boh, L. (2006). Groups perform better than the best individuals on letters-to-numbers problems: effects of group size. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 90(4), 644–651.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Leidner, D. E., & Fuller, M. (1997). Improving student learning of conceptual information: GSS supported collaborative learning vs. individual constructive learning. Decision Support Systems, 20(2), 149–163.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Lorge, I., Fox, D., Davitz, J., & Brenner, M. (1958). A survey of studies contrasting the quality of group performance and individual performance. Psychological Bulletin, 55, 337–372.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Lou, Y., Abrami, P. C., & d'Apollonia, S. (2001). Small group and individual learning with technology: a meta-analysis. Review of Educational Research, 71(3), 449–521.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Meade, M. L., Nokes, T. J., & Morrow, D. G. (2009). Expertise promotes facilitation on a collaborative memory task. Memory, 17(1), 39–48.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Meudell, P. R., Hitch, G. J., & Boyle, M. M. (1995). Collaboration in recall: do pairs of people cross-cue each other to produce new memories? The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 48, 141–152.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Mullen, B. (1983). Operationalizing the effect of the group on the individual: a self-attention perspective. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 19(4), 295–322.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Mullen, B. (1987). Self-attention theory. In B. Mullen & G. R. Goethals (Eds.), Theories of group behaviour (pp. 125–146). New York: Springer-Verlag.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Nokes-Malach, T. J., Meade, M. L., & Morrow, D. G. (2012). The effect of expertise on collaborative problem solving. Thinking and Reasoning, 18(1), 32–58.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Okada, T., & Simon, H. A. (1997). Collaborative discovery in a scientific domain. Cognitive Science, 21(2), 109–146.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Paulus, P. B., & Yang, H. C. (2000). Idea generation in groups: a basis for creativity in organizations. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 82, 76–87.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. Rajaram, S., & Pereira-Pasarin, L. P. (2010). Collaborative memory: cognitive research and theory. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 5(6), 649–663.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  57. Roediger, H. L., & Karpicke, J. D. (2006). The power of testing memory: basic research and implications for educational practice. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 1, 181–210.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  58. Roschelle, J., & Teasley, S. D. (1995). The construction of shared knowledge in collaborative problem solving. In C. O’Mally (Ed.), Computer supported collaborative learning (pp. 69–97). Berlin Heidelberg: Springer-Verlag.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  59. Ross, M., Spencer, S. J., Linardatos, L., Lam, K. C. H., & Perunovic, M. (2004). Going shopping and identifying landmarks: does collaboration improve older people’s memory? Applied Cognitive Psychology, 18(6), 683–696.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  60. Schwartz, D. L. (1999). The productive agency that drives collaborative learning. In P. Dillenbourg (Ed.), Collaborative learning: Cognitive and computational approaches (pp. 197–218). Oxford: Elsevier.Google Scholar
  61. Slavin, R. E. (1980). Cooperative learning. Review of Educational Research, 50(2), 315–342.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  62. Slavin, R. E. (1995). Cooperative learning: Theory, research, and practice (2nd ed.). Boston: Allyn & Bacon.Google Scholar
  63. Slavin, R. E. (2014). Making cooperative learning powerful. Educational Leadership, 72(2), 22–26.Google Scholar
  64. Springer, L., Stanne, M. E., & Donovan, S. S. (1999). Effects of small-group learning on undergraduates in science, mathematics, engineering, and technology: a meta-analysis. Review of Educational Research, 69(1), 21–51.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  65. Steiner, I. D. (1966). Models for inferring the relationships between group size and potential group productivity. Behavioral Science, 11, 273–283.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  66. Steiner, I. D. (1972). Group processes and productivity. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  67. Teasley, S. D. (1995). The role of talk in children’s peer collaborations. Developmental Psychology, 31(2), 207–220.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  68. Thorley, C., & Dewhurst, S. A. (2009). False and veridical collaborative recognition. Memory, 17, 17–25.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  69. Tudge, J. (1989). When collaboration leads to regression: some negative consequences of socio-cognitive conflict. European Journal of Social Psychology, 19(2), 123–138.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  70. Voiklis, J., & Corter, J. E. (2012). Conventional wisdom: negotiating conventions of reference enhances category learning. Cognitive Science, 36(4), 607–634.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  71. Webb, N. M., Troper, J. D., & Fall, R. (1995). Constructive activity and learning in collaborative small groups. Journal of Educational Psychology, 87, 406–423.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  72. Weldon, M. S., & Bellinger, K. D. (1997). Collective memory: collaborative and individual processes in remembering. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 23(5), 1160–1175.Google Scholar
  73. Wright, D. B. (2007). Calculating nominal group statistics in collaboration studies. Behavior Research Methods, 39, 460–470.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media New York 2015

Authors and Affiliations

  • Timothy J. Nokes-Malach
    • 1
    Email author
  • J. Elizabeth Richey
    • 1
  • Soniya Gadgil
    • 1
  1. 1.Department of Psychology, Learning Research and Development CenterUniversity of PittsburghPittsburghUSA

Personalised recommendations