Advertisement

Educational Psychology Review

, Volume 26, Issue 2, pp 191–195 | Cite as

Cognitive Load Theory: A Broader View on the Role of Memory in Learning and Education

  • Fred PaasEmail author
  • Paul Ayres
Review Article

Abstract

According to cognitive load theory (CLT), the limitations of working memory (WM) in the learning of new tasks together with its ability to cooperate with an unlimited long-term memory (LTM) for familiar tasks enable human beings to deal effectively with complex problems and acquire highly complex knowledge and skills. With regard to WM, CLT has focused to a large extent on learning task characteristics, and to a lesser extent on learner characteristics to manage WM load and optimize learning through instructional design. With regard to LTM, explanations of human learning and cognition have mainly focused on domain-general skills, instead of domain-specific knowledge held in LTM. The contributions to this special issue provide a broader cognitive load view on the role of memory in learning and education by presenting the historical roots and conceptual development of the concept of WM, as well as the theoretical and practical implications of current debates about WM mechanisms (Cowan 2014), by presenting an updated model of cognitive load in which the physical learning environment is considered a distinct causal factor for WM load (Choi et al. 2014), by an experimental demonstration of the effects of persistent pain on the available WM resources for learning (Smith and Ayres 2014), and by using aspects of evolutionary educational psychology to argue for the primacy of domain-specific knowledge in human cognition (Tricot and Sweller 2014).

Keywords

Cognitive load theory Working memory Learning Education Instructional design 

References

  1. Antonenko, P. D., Paas, F., Grabner, R., & Van Gog, T. (2010). Using electroencephalography to measure of cognitive load. Educ Psychol Rev, 22, 425–438.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Ayres, P. (1993). Why goal-free problems can facilitate learning. Contemp Educ Psychol, 18, 376–381.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Ayres, P., & Sweller, J. (2005). The split-attention principle in multimedia learning. In R. E. Mayer (Ed.), The Cambridge handbook of multimedia learning (pp. 135–146). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Ayres, P., & Sweller, J. (in press). The split-attention principle in multimedia learning. In R. E. Mayer (Ed.), The Cambridge handbook of multimedia learning, 2 nd Edition. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  5. Baddeley, A. D. (1986). Working memory. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  6. Baddeley, A. D., & Hitch, G. J. (1974). Working memory. In G. Bower (Ed.), Recent advances in learning and motivation (Vol. VIII, pp. 47–90). New York, NY: Academic.Google Scholar
  7. Bartlett, F. C. (1932). Remembering: a study in experimental and social psychology. Oxford, England: Macmillan.Google Scholar
  8. Chandler, P., & Sweller, J. (1992). The split–attention effect as a factor in the design of instruction. Br J Educ Psychol, 62, 233–246.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Chi, M., Glaser, R., & Rees, E. (1982). Expertise in problem solving. In R. Sternberg (Ed.), Advances in the psychology of human intelligence (pp. 7–75). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  10. Choi, H. H., Van Merriënboer, J. J. G., Paas, F. (2014). Effects of the physical environment on cognitive load and learning: towards a new model of cognitive load. Educational Psychology Review. Google Scholar
  11. Cowan, N. (1988). Evolving conceptions of memory storage, selective attention, and their mutual constraints within the human information processing system. Psychol Bull, 104, 163–191.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Cowan, N. (2001). The magical number 4 in short-term memory: a reconsideration of mental storage capacity. Behav Brain Sci, 24, 87–114.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Cowan, N. (2014). Working memory underpins cognitive development, learning, and education. Educational Psychology Review. Google Scholar
  14. Dick, B. D., & Rashiq, S. (2007). Disruption of attention and working memory traces in individuals with chronic pain. Anesthesia & Analgesia, 104, 1223–1229.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Ericsson, K. A., & Charness, N. (1994). Expert performance: its structure and acquisition. Am Psychol, 49, 725–747.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Eysenck, M. W. (1985). Anxiety and cognitive-task performance. Personal Individ Differ, 6, 574–586.Google Scholar
  17. Fraser, K., Ma, I., Teteris, E., Baxter, H., Wright, B., & McLaughlin, K. (2012). Emotion, cognitive load and learning outcomes during simulation training. Med Educ, 46, 1055–1062.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Geary, D. C. (2008). An evolutionarily informed education science. Educ Psychol, 43, 179–195.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Geary, D. (2012). Evolutionary educational psychology. In K. Harris, S. Graham, & T. Urdan (Eds.), APA educational psychology handbook (Vol. 1, pp. 597–621). Washington, D.C.: American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
  20. Kalyuga, S. (2007). Expertise reversal effect and its implications for learner-tailored instruction. Educ Psychol Rev, 19, 509–539.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Kalyuga, S., Chandler, P., & Sweller, J. (1999). Managing split-attention and redundancy in multimedia instruction. Appl Cogn Psychol, 13, 351–371.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Kalyuga, S., Ayres, P., Chandler, P., & Sweller, J. (2003). The expertise reversal effect. Educ Psychol, 38, 23–31.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Kalyuga, S., Rikers, R., & Paas, F. (2012). Educational implications of expertise reversal effects in learning and performance of complex cognitive and sensorimotor skills. Educ Psychol Rev, 24, 313–337.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Kotovsky, K., Hayes, J. R., & Simon, H. A. (1985). Why are some problems hard? Evidence from Tower of Hanoi. Cogn Psychol, 17, 248–294.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Liu, T.–. C., Lin, Y.–. C., Tsai, M.-J., & Paas, F. (2011). Split-attention and redundancy effects on mobile learning in physical environments. Comput Educ, 58, 172–180.Google Scholar
  26. Liu, T. C., Lin, Y. C., & Paas, F. (2014). Effects of prior knowledge on learning from different compositions of representations in a mobile learning environment. Comput Educ, 72, 328–338.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Miller, G. (1956). The magical number seven, plus or minus two: some limits on our capacity for processing information. Psychol Rev, 63, 81–97.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Paas, F., & Sweller, J. (2012). An evolutionary upgrade of cognitive load theory: using the human motor system and collaboration to support the learning of complex cognitive tasks. Educ Psychol Rev, 24, 27–45.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Paas, F., Camp, G., & Rikers, R. (2001). Instructional compensation for age-related cognitive declines: effects of goal specificity in maze learning. J Educ Psychol, 93, 181.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Paas, F., Renkl, A., & Sweller, J. (2003). Cognitive load theory and instructional design: recent developments. Educ Psychol, 38, 1–4.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Peterson, L., & Peterson, M. J. (1959). Short-term retention of individual verbal items. J Exp Psychol, 58, 193–198.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Ramirez, G., & Beilock, S. L. (2011). Writing about testing worries boosts exam performance in the classroom. Science, 331(6014), 211–213.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Schneider, W., & Shiffrin, R. M. (1977). Controlled and automatic human information processing: I. Detection, search, and attention. Psychol Rev, 84, 1–66.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Simon, H. A., & Gilmartin, K. (1973). A simulation of memory for chess positions. Cogn Psychol, 5, 29–46.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Smith, A., & Ayres, P. (2014). The impact of persistent pain on working memory and learning. Educational Psychology Review.Google Scholar
  36. Sweller, J. (1988). Cognitive load during problem solving: effects on learning. Cogn Sci, 12, 257–285.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Sweller, J. (2008). Instructional implications of David C. Geary's evolutionary educational psychology. Educ Psychol, 43, 214–216.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Sweller, J., & Chandler, P. (1991). Evidence for cognitive load theory. Cogn Instr, 8, 351–362.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Sweller, J., Van Merriënboer, J. J. G., & Paas, F. (1998). Cognitive architecture and instructional design. Educ Psychol Rev, 10, 251–296.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Sweller, J., Ayres, P., & Kalyuga, S. (2011). Cognitive load theory (vol. 1). New York: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Tricot, A., & Sweller, J. (2014). Domain-specific knowledge and why teaching generic skills does not work. Educational Psychology ReviewGoogle Scholar
  42. Van Gerven, P. W. M., Paas, F., Van Merriënboer, J. J. G., & Schmidt, H. G. (2002). Cognitive load theory and aging: effects of worked examples on training efficiency. Learn Instr, 12, 87–105.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Van Gerven, P. W. M., Paas, F., Van Merriënboer, J. J. G., & Schmidt, H. G. (2004). Memory load and task-evoked pupillary responses in aging. Psychophysiology, 41, 167–175.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Vredeveldt, A., Hitch, G., & Baddeley, A. D. (2011). Eyeclosure helps memory by reducing cognitive load and enhancing visualisation. Memory & cognition, 39, 1253–1263.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media New York 2014

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Institute of PsychologyErasmus University RotterdamRotterdamThe Netherlands
  2. 2.Early Start Research InstituteUniversity of WollongongWollongongAustralia
  3. 3.School of EducationUniversity of New South WalesSydneyAustralia

Personalised recommendations