Educational Psychology Review

, Volume 26, Issue 2, pp 197–223 | Cite as

Working Memory Underpins Cognitive Development, Learning, and Education

Review Article

Abstract

Working memory is the retention of a small amount of information in a readily accessible form. It facilitates planning, comprehension, reasoning, and problem solving. I examine the historical roots and conceptual development of the concept and the theoretical and practical implications of current debates about working memory mechanisms. Then, I explore the nature of cognitive developmental improvements in working memory, the role of working memory in learning, and some potential implications of working memory and its development for the education of children and adults. The use of working memory is quite ubiquitous in human thought, but the best way to improve education using what we know about working memory is still controversial. I hope to provide some directions for research and educational practice.

Keywords

Working memory Cognitive development Learning Education 

References

  1. Andrews, G., & Halford, G. S. (2002). A cognitive complexity metric applied to cognitive development. Cognitive Psychology, 45, 153–219.Google Scholar
  2. Atkinson, R. C., & Shiffrin, R. M. (1968). Human memory: a proposed system and its control processes. In K. W. Spence & J. T. Spence (Eds.), The psychology of learning and motivation: advances in research and theory (Vol. 2, pp. 89–195). New York: Academic.Google Scholar
  3. Baddeley, A. D. (1986). Working memory. Oxford Psychology Series #11. Oxford: Clarendon.Google Scholar
  4. Baddeley, A. (2000). The episodic buffer: a new component of working memory? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4, 417–423.Google Scholar
  5. Baddeley, A. D., Gathercole, S. E., & Papagno, C. (1998). The phonological loop as a language learning device. Psychological Review, 105, 158–173.Google Scholar
  6. Barrouillet, P., Gavens, N., Vergauwe, E., Gaillard, V., & Camos, V. (2009). Working memory span development: a time-based resource-sharing model account. Developmental Psychology, 45, 477–490.Google Scholar
  7. Baddeley, A. D., & Hitch, G. (1974). Working memory. In G. H. Bower (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 8, pp. 47–89). New York: Academic.Google Scholar
  8. Baddeley, A., Papagno, C., & Vallar, G. (1988). When long-term learning depends on short-term storage. Journal of Memory and Language, 27, 586–595.Google Scholar
  9. Barrouillet, P., Portrat, S., & Camos, V. (2011). On the law relating processing to storage in working memory. Psychological Review, 118, 175–192.Google Scholar
  10. Bauer, P. J., & Fivush, R. (2013). Handbook on the development of children’s memory. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  11. Bolton, T. L. (1892). The growth of memory in school children. American Journal of Psychology, 4, 362–380.Google Scholar
  12. Borst, G., Niven, E., & Logie, R. H. (2012). Visual mental image generation does not overlap with visual short-term memory: a dual-task interference study. Memory & cognition, 40, 360–372.Google Scholar
  13. Broadbent, D. E. (1958). Perception and communication. New York: Pergamon Press.Google Scholar
  14. Brown, G. D. A., Neath, I., & Chater, N. (2007). A temporal ratio model of memory. Psychological Review, 114, 539–576.Google Scholar
  15. Burgess, N., & Hitch, G. J. (1999). Memory for serial order: a network model of the phonological loop and its timing. Psychological Review, 106, 551–581.Google Scholar
  16. Camos, V., & Barrouillet, P. (2011). Developmental change in working memory strategies: from passive maintenance to active refreshing. Developmental Psychology, 47, 898–904.Google Scholar
  17. Camos, V., Mora, G., & Oberauer, K. (2011). Adaptive choice between articulatory rehearsal and attentional refreshing in verbal working memory. Memory & Cognition, 39, 231–244.Google Scholar
  18. Case, R., Kurland, D. M., & Goldberg, J. (1982). Operational efficiency and the growth of short-term memory span. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 33, 386–404.Google Scholar
  19. Chein, J., & Morrison, A. (2010). Expanding the mind’s workspace: training and transfer effects with a complex working memory span task. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 17, 193–199.Google Scholar
  20. Chen, Z., & Cowan, N. (2009). Core verbal working memory capacity: the limit in words retained without covert articulation. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 62, 1420–1429.Google Scholar
  21. Chen, Z., & Cowan, N. (2013). Working memory inefficiency: minimal information is utilized in visual recognition tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 39, 1449–1462.Google Scholar
  22. Chi, M. T. H. (1978). Knowledge structures and memory development. In R. Siegler (Ed.), Children’s thinking: what develops? Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  23. Clark, E. V., & Garnica, O. K. (1974). Is he coming or going? On the acquisition of deictic verbs. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 13, 559–572.Google Scholar
  24. Cocchini, G., Logie, R. H., Della Sala, S., MacPherson, S. E., & Baddeley, A. D. (2002). Concurrent performance of two memory tasks: evidence for domain-specific working memory systems. Memory & Cognition, 30, 1086–1095.Google Scholar
  25. Conrad, R. (1964). Acoustic confusion in immediate memory. British Journal of Psychology, 55, 75–84.Google Scholar
  26. Courage, M. L., & Cowan, N. (Eds.). (2009). The development of memory in infancy and childhood. Hove, UK: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
  27. Cowan, N. (1988). Evolving conceptions of memory storage, selective attention, and their mutual constraints within the human information processing system. Psychological Bulletin, 104, 163–191.Google Scholar
  28. Cowan, N. (1992). Verbal memory span and the timing of spoken recall. Journal of Memory and Language, 31, 668–684.Google Scholar
  29. Cowan, N. (1995). Attention and memory: an integrated framework. Oxford Psychology Series, no. 26. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  30. Cowan, N. (1999). An embedded-processes model of working memory. In A. Miyake & P. Shah (Eds.), Models of working memory: mechanisms of active maintenance and executive control (pp. 62–101). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  31. Cowan, N. (2001). The magical number 4 in short-term memory: a reconsideration of mental storage capacity. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24, 87–185.Google Scholar
  32. Cowan, N. (2005). Working memory capacity. Hove, East Sussex, UK: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
  33. Cowan, N. (2010). The magical mystery four: how is working memory capacity limited, and why? Current Directions in Psychological Science, 19, 51–57.Google Scholar
  34. Cowan, N. (2011). The focus of attention as observed in visual working memory tasks: making sense of competing claims. Neuropsychologia, 49, 1401–1406.Google Scholar
  35. Cowan, N., AuBuchon, A. M., Gilchrist, A. L., Ricker, T. J., & Saults, J. S. (2011a). Age differences in visual working memory capacity: not based on encoding limitations. Developmental Science, 14, 1066–1074.Google Scholar
  36. Cowan, N., Donnell, K., & Saults, J. S. (2013). A list-length constraint on incidental item-to-item associations. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. doi:10.3758/s13423-013-0447-7.Google Scholar
  37. Cowan, N., Li, D., Moffitt, A., Becker, T. M., Martin, E. A., Saults, J. S., et al. (2011b). A neural region of abstract working memory. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23, 2852–2863.Google Scholar
  38. Cowan, N., & Morey, C. C. (2007). How can dual-task working memory retention limits be investigated? Psychological Science, 18, 686–688.Google Scholar
  39. Cowan, N., Morey, C. C., AuBuchon, A. M., Zwilling, C. E., & Gilchrist, A. L. (2010). Seven-year-olds allocate attention like adults unless working memory is overloaded. Developmental Science, 13, 120–133.Google Scholar
  40. Cowan, N., Nugent, L. D., Elliott, E. M., Ponomarev, I., & Saults, J. S. (1999). The role of attention in the development of short-term memory: age differences in the verbal span of apprehension. Child Development, 70, 1082–1097.Google Scholar
  41. Cowan, N., Rouder, J. N., Blume, C. L., & Saults, J. S. (2012). Models of verbal working memory capacity: what does it take to make them work? Psychological Review, 119, 480–499.Google Scholar
  42. Cowan, N., Saults, J. S., Winterowd, C., & Sherk, M. (1991). Enhancement of 4-year-old children’s memory span for phonologically similar and dissimilar word lists. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 51, 30–52.Google Scholar
  43. Craik, F. I. M., & Watkins, M. J. (1973). The role of rehearsal in short-term memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 12, 599–607.Google Scholar
  44. Daneman, M., & Carpenter, P. A. (1980). Individual differences in working memory and reading. Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior, 19, 450–466.Google Scholar
  45. Diamond, A., & Lee, K. (2011). Interventions shown to aid executive function development in children 4 to 12 years old. Science, 333, 959–964.Google Scholar
  46. Ebbinghaus, H. (1885/1913). Memory: a contribution to experimental psychology. Translated by H.A. Ruger & C.E. Bussenius. New York: Teachers College, Columbia University (originally in German, Ueber das gedächtnis: Untersuchen zur experimentellen psychologie)Google Scholar
  47. Engle, R. W., Tuholski, S. W., Laughlin, J. E., & Conway, A. R. A. (1999). Working memory, short term memory, and general fluid intelligence: a latent variable approach. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 128, 309–331.Google Scholar
  48. Ericsson, K. A., Chase, W. G., & Faloon, S. (1980). Acquisition of a memory skill. Science, 208, 1181–1182.Google Scholar
  49. Ericsson, K. A., Delaney, P. F., Weaver, G., & Mahadevan, R. (2004). Uncovering the structure of a memorist’s superior basic-memory capacity. Cognitive Psychology, 49, 191–237.Google Scholar
  50. Ericsson, K. A., & Kintsch, W. (1995). Long-term working-memory. Psychological Review, 102, 211–245.Google Scholar
  51. Farrell, S., & Lewandowsky, S. (2002). An endogenous distributed model of ordering in serial recall. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 9, 59–79.Google Scholar
  52. Flavell, J. H., Beach, D. H., & Chinsky, J. M. (1966). Spontaneous verbal rehearsal in a memory task as a function of age. Child Development, 37, 283–299.Google Scholar
  53. Fougnie, D., & Marois, R. (2011). What limits working memory capacity? Evidence for modality-specific sources to the simultaneous storage of visual and auditory arrays. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 37, 1329–1341.Google Scholar
  54. Garcia, L., Nussbaum, M., & Preiss, D. D. (2011). Is the use of information and communication technology related to performance in working memory tasks? Evidence from seventh-grade students. Computers & Education, 57, 2068–2076.Google Scholar
  55. Garrity, L. I. (1975). An electromyographical study of subvocal speech and recall in preschool children. Developmental Psychology, 11, 274–281.Google Scholar
  56. Gathercole, S. E., & Baddeley, A. D. (1989). Evaluation of the role of phonological STM in the development of vocabulary in children: a longitudinal study. Journal of Memory and Language, 28, 200–213.Google Scholar
  57. Gathercole, S. E., & Baddeley, A. D. (1990). Phonological memory deficits in language disordered children: is there a causal connection? Journal of Memory and Language, 29, 336–360.Google Scholar
  58. Gathercole, S. E., Lamont, E., & Alloway, T. P. (2006). Working memory in the classroom. In S. J. Pickering (Ed.), Working memory and education (pp. 219–240). San Diego: Academic.Google Scholar
  59. Gathercole, S. E., Pickering, S. J., Ambridge, B., & Wearing, H. (2004). The structure of working memory from 4 to 15 years of age. Developmental Psychology, 40, 177–190.Google Scholar
  60. Geary, D. C. (2008). An evolutionarily informed education science. Educational Psychologist, 43, 179–195.Google Scholar
  61. Gelman, R. (1982). Accessing one‐to‐one correspondence: still another paper about conservation. British Journal of Psychology, 73, 209–220.Google Scholar
  62. Gilchrist, A. L., Cowan, N., & Naveh-Benjamin, M. (2009). Investigating the childhood development of working memory using sentences: new evidence for the growth of chunk capacity. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 104, 252–265.Google Scholar
  63. Halford, G. S., Cowan, N., & Andrews, G. (2007). Separating cognitive capacity from knowledge: a new hypothesis. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11, 236–242.Google Scholar
  64. Halford, G. S., Wilson, W. H., & Phillips, S. (1998). Processing capacity defined by relational complexity: implications for comparative, developmental, and cognitive psychology. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 21, 803–865.Google Scholar
  65. Hall, V. C. (2003). Educational psychology from 1890 to 1920. In B. J. Zimmerman & D. H. Schunk (Eds.), Educational psychology: a century of contributions. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  66. Halpern, D. F. (1998). Teaching critical thinking for transfer across domains: dispositions, skills, structure training, and metacognitive monitoring. American Psychologist, 53, 449–455.Google Scholar
  67. Halpern, D. F., Millis, K., Graesser, A. C., Butler, H., Forsyth, C., & Cai, Z. (2012). Operation ARA: a computerized learning game that teaches critical thinking and scientific reasoning. Thinking Skills and Creativity, 7, 93–100.Google Scholar
  68. Hebb, D. O. (1949). Organization of behavior. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  69. Henry, L. A. (1991). The effects of word length and phonemic similarity in young children’s short-term memory. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 43A, 35–52.Google Scholar
  70. Henson, R. N. A. (1998). Short-term memory for serial order: the start–end model. Cognitive Psychology, 36, 73–137.Google Scholar
  71. Hulme, C., & Tordoff, V. (1989). Working memory development: the effects of speech rate, word length, and acoustic similarity on serial recall. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 47, 72–87.Google Scholar
  72. Jaeggi, S. M., & Buschkuehl, M. (2013). Training working memory. In T. P. Alloway & R. G. Alloway (Eds.), Working memory: the connected intelligence. New York: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
  73. James, W. (1890). The principles of psychology. New York: Henry Holt.Google Scholar
  74. Jarrold, C., & Citroën, R. (2013). Reevaluating key evidence for the development of rehearsal: phonological similarity effects in children are subject to proportional scaling artifacts. Developmental Psychology, 49, 837–847.Google Scholar
  75. Jevons, W. S. (1871). The power of numerical discrimination. Nature, 3, 281–282.Google Scholar
  76. Jolicoeur, P., & Dell’Acqua, R. (1998). The demonstration of short-term consolidation. Cognitive Psychology, 36, 138–202.Google Scholar
  77. Kail, R. (1990). The development of memory in children (3rd ed.). New York: W.H. Freeman.Google Scholar
  78. Kail, R., & Salthouse, T. A. (1994). Processing speed as a mental capacity. Acta Psychologica, 86, 199–255.Google Scholar
  79. Kane, M. J., Brown, L. H., McVay, J. C., Silvia, P. J., Myin-Germeys, I., & Kwapil, T. R. (2007). For whom the mind wanders, and when: an experience–sampling study of working memory and executive control in daily life. Psychological Science, 18, 614–621.Google Scholar
  80. Kane, M. J., & Engle, R. W. (2003). Working-memory capacity and the control of attention: the contributions of goal neglect, response competition, and task set to Stroop interference. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 132, 47–70.Google Scholar
  81. Kane, M. J., Hambrick, D. Z., Tuholski, S. W., Wilhelm, O., Payne, T. W., & Engle, R. W. (2004). The generality of working memory capacity: a latent variable approach to verbal and visuospatial memory span and reasoning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 133, 189–217.Google Scholar
  82. Klingberg, T. (2010). Training and plasticity of working memory. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 14, 317–324.Google Scholar
  83. Kyllonen, P. C., & Christal, R. E. (1990). Reasoning ability is (little more than) working-memory capacity?! Intelligence, 14, 389–433.Google Scholar
  84. Kyndt, E., Cascallar, E., & Dochy, F. (2012). Individual differences in working memory capacity and attention, and their relationship with students’ approaches to learning. Higher Education: The International Journal of Higher Education and Educational Planning, 64, 285–297.Google Scholar
  85. Lewandowsky, S., Duncan, M., & Brown, G. D. A. (2004). Time does not cause forgetting in short-term serial recall. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 11, 771–790.Google Scholar
  86. Locke, J. (1690). An essay concerning human understanding. London: Thomas Bassett.Google Scholar
  87. Logie, R. H. (1996). The seven ages of working memory. In J. T. E. Richardson, R. W. Engle, L. Hasher, R. H. Logie, E. R. Stoltzfus, & R. T. Zacks (Eds.), Working memory and human cognition (pp. 31–65). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  88. Logie, R. H., & Van Der Meulen, M. (2009). Fragmenting and integrating visuospatial working memory. In J. R. Brockmole (Ed.), Representing the visual world in memory (pp. 1–32). Hove, UK: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
  89. Luck, S. J., & Vogel, E. K. (1997). The capacity of visual working memory for features and conjunctions. Nature, 390, 279–281.Google Scholar
  90. Mandler, G., & Shebo, B. J. (1982). Subitizing: an analysis of its component processes. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 111, 1–22.Google Scholar
  91. Melby-Lervåg, M., & Hulme, C. (2013). Is working memory training effective? A meta-analytic review. Developmental Psychology, 49, 270–291.Google Scholar
  92. Miller, G. A. (1956). The magical number seven, plus or minus two: some limits on our capacity for processing information. Psychological Review, 63, 81–97.Google Scholar
  93. Miller, G. A., Galanter, E., & Pribram, K. H. (1960). Plans and the structure of behavior. New York: Holt.Google Scholar
  94. Morey, C. C., & Bieler, M. (2013). Visual short-term memory always requires attention. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 20, 163–170.Google Scholar
  95. Murdock, B. B. (1983). A distributed memory model for serial-order information. Psychological Review, 90, 316–338.Google Scholar
  96. Murdock, B. B., & Walker, K. D. (1969). Modality effects in free recall. Journal of Verbal Leaning and Verbal Behavior, 8, 665–676.Google Scholar
  97. Nelson, K. J. (1974). Variations in children’s concepts by age and category. Child Development, 45, 577–584.Google Scholar
  98. Norman, D. A. (1968). Toward a theory of memory and attention. Psychological Review, 75(6), 522–536.Google Scholar
  99. Oberauer, K., & Lewandowsky, S. (2008). Forgetting in immediate serial recall: decay, temporal distinctiveness, or interference? Psychological Review, 115, 544–576.Google Scholar
  100. Oberauer, K., & Lewandowsky, S. (2011). Modeling working memory: a computational implementation of the time-based resource-sharing theory. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 18, 10–45.Google Scholar
  101. Oberauer, K., Lewandowsky, S., Farrell, S., Jarrold, C., & Greaves, M. (2012). Modeling working memory: an interference model of complex span. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 19, 779–819.Google Scholar
  102. Ornstein, P. A., & Naus, M. J. (1978). Rehearsal processes in children’s memory. In P. A. Ornstein (Ed.), Memory development in children (pp. 69–99). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  103. Paas, F., & Sweller, J. (2012). An evolutionary upgrade of cognitive load theory: using the human motor system and collaboration to support the learning of complex cognitive tasks. Educational Psychology Review, 24, 27–45.Google Scholar
  104. Pascual-Leone, J., & Johnson, J. (2011). A developmental theory of mental attention: its applications to measurement and task analysis. In P. Barrouillet & V. Gaillard (Eds.), Cognitive development and working memory: from neoPiagetian to cognitive approaches (pp. 13–46). Hove, UK: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
  105. Pascual-Leone, J., & Smith, J. (1969). The encoding and decoding of symbols by children: a new experimental paradigm and a neo-Piagetian model. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 8, 328–355.Google Scholar
  106. Penney, C. G. (1989). Modality effects and the structure of short-term verbal memory. Memory & Cognition, 17, 398–422.Google Scholar
  107. Pickering, S. J. (2006). Working memory and education. San Diego: Academic.Google Scholar
  108. Redick, T. S., Shipstead, Z., Harrison, T. L., Hicks, K. L., Fried, D. E., Hambrick, D. Z., et al. (2013). No evidence of intelligence improvement after working memory training: a randomized, placebo-controlled study. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 142, 359–379.Google Scholar
  109. Ricker, T. J., & Cowan, N. (2010). Loss of visual working memory within seconds: the combined use of refreshable and non-refreshable features. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 36, 1355–1368.Google Scholar
  110. Ricker, T.J., & Cowan, N. (2013). Differences between presentation methods in working memory procedures: a matter of working memory consolidation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition. doi:10.1037/a0034301.
  111. Rouder, J. N., Morey, R. D., Cowan, N., Zwilling, C. E., Morey, C. C., & Pratte, M. S. (2008). An assessment of fixed-capacity models of visual working memory. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences (PNAS), 105, 5975–5979.Google Scholar
  112. Sabol, T. J., & Pianta, R. C. (2012). Patterns of school readiness forecast achievement and socioemotional development at the end of elementary school. Child Development, 83, 282–299.Google Scholar
  113. Saltz, E., Soller, E., & Sigel, I. E. (1972). The development of natural language concepts. Child Development, 1972(43), 1191–1202.Google Scholar
  114. Schüler, A., Scheiter, K., & Genuchten, E. (2011). The role of working memory in multimedia instruction: is working memory working during learning from text and pictures? Educational Psychology Review, 23, 389–411.Google Scholar
  115. Shim, W., & Walczak, K. (2012). The impact of faculty teaching practices on the development of students’ critical thinking skills. International Journal of Teaching and Learning in Higher Education, 24, 16–30.Google Scholar
  116. Shipstead, Z., Redick, T. S., & Engle, R. W. (2012). Is working memory training effective? Psychological Bulletin, 138, 628–654.Google Scholar
  117. Slevc, L. (2011). Saying what’s on your mind: working memory effects on sentence production. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 37, 1503–1514.Google Scholar
  118. Sokolov, E. N. (1963). Perception and the conditioned reflex. New York: Pergamon Press.Google Scholar
  119. Stanovich, K. E., West, R. F., & Toplak, M. E. (2013). Myside bias, rational thinking, and intelligence. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 22, 259–264.Google Scholar
  120. Süß, H. M., Oberauer, K., Wittmann, W. W., Wilhelm, O., & Schulze, R. (2002). Working-memory capacity explains reasoning ability—and a little bit more. Intelligence, 30, 261–288.Google Scholar
  121. Sweller, J. (2011). Cognitive load theory. Psychology of Learning and Motivation, 55, 37–76.Google Scholar
  122. Sweller, J., van Merrienboer, J. J. G., & Paas, F. G. W. C. (1998). Cognitive architecture and instructional design. Educational Psychology Review, 10, 251–296.Google Scholar
  123. Tan, L., & Ward, G. (2000). A recency-based account of the primacy effect in free recall. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 26, 1589–1625.Google Scholar
  124. Todd, J. J., & Marois, R. (2004). Capacity limit of visual short-term memory in human posterior parietal cortex. Nature, 428, 751–754.Google Scholar
  125. Treisman, A. M. (1960). Contextual cues in selective listening. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 12, 242–248.Google Scholar
  126. Tricot, A., & Sweller, J. (2013). Domain-specific knowledge and why teaching generic skills does not work. Educational Psychology Review. doi:10.1007/s10648-013-9243-1.Google Scholar
  127. Van Der Meulen, M., Logie, R. H., & Sala, S. D. (2009). Selective interference with image retention and generation: evidence for the workspace model. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 62, 1568–1580.Google Scholar
  128. Vergauwe, E., Barrouillet, P., & Camos, V. (2010). Do mental processes share a domain-general resource? Psychological Science, 21, 384–390.Google Scholar
  129. Vogel, E. K., McCollough, A. W., & Machizawa, M. G. (2005). Neural measures reveal individual differences in controlling access to working memory. Nature, 438, 500–503.Google Scholar
  130. Wason, P. C., & Shapiro, D. (1971). Natural and contrived experience in a reasoning problem. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 23, 63–71.Google Scholar
  131. Wilding, J. (2001). Over the top: are there exceptions to the basic capacity limit? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24, 152–153.Google Scholar
  132. Wilhelm, O., & Engle, R. W. (Eds.). (2005). Handbook of understanding and measuring intelligence. London: Sage.Google Scholar
  133. Wundt, W. (1894/1998). Lectures on human and animal psychology. Translated from the second German edition by J.E. Creighton & E.B. Titchener. Bristol, UK: Thoemmes Press.Google Scholar
  134. Xu, Y., & Chun, M. M. (2006). Dissociable neural mechanisms supporting visual short-term memory for objects. Nature, 440, 91–95.Google Scholar
  135. Zhang, W., & Luck, S. J. (2009). Sudden death and gradual decay in visual working memory. Psychological Science, 20, 423–428.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media New York 2013

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of Psychological SciencesUniversity of MissouriColumbiaUSA

Personalised recommendations