Educational Psychology Review

, Volume 25, Issue 3, pp 391–427 | Cite as

Relational Reasoning and Its Manifestations in the Educational Context: a Systematic Review of the Literature

  • Denis Dumas
  • Patricia A. Alexander
  • Emily M. Grossnickle
Original Paper

Abstract

Relational reasoning, the ability to discern meaningful patterns within otherwise unconnected information, is regarded as central to human learning and cognition and as particularly critical for those functioning in today’s information age. However, the literature on this foundational ability is currently housed within a range of domains of inquiry, where divergent terminology and methodologies are commonplace. This dispersion has made it difficult to harness the power of existing work to inform future research or guide educational practice. In order to address this lack of consolidation, a systematic review of relational reasoning was undertaken. Specifically, 109 empirical studies dealing with relational reasoning in general or one of four manifestations (i.e., analogy, anomaly, antinomy, and antithesis) were analyzed. Resulting data revealed trends across fields of inquiry, including a degree of conceptual ambiguity, conceptual and operational misalignment, and a lack of ecological validity in certain research paradigms. There were also particular forms and measures of relational reasoning that were more commonly investigated, as well as certain domains that were more often studied. Implications for how future research can examine relational reasoning as a multidimensional construct within educational contexts are also discussed.

Keywords

Relational reasoning Analogy Anomaly Antinomy Antithesis 

Supplementary material

10648_2013_9224_MOESM1_ESM.pdf (82 kb)
Table 1Relational reasoning in general literature summary table (PDF 81.7 kb)
10648_2013_9224_MOESM2_ESM.pdf (232 kb)
Table 2Particular manifestations of relational reasoning literature summary table (PDF 232 kb)

References

  1. Acredolo, C., & Horobin, K. (1987). Development of relational reasoning and avoidance of premature closure. Developmental Psychology, 23(1), 13–21. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.23.1.13.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Afflerbach, P., & Cho, B.-Y. (2009). Identifying and describing constructively responsive comprehension strategies in new and traditional forms of reading. In S. E. Israel & G. G. Duffy (Eds.), Handbook of research on reading comprehension (pp. 69–114). New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
  3. Alexander, P. A., & The Disciplined Reading and Learning Research Laboratory. (2012). Reading into the future: competence for the 21st century. Educational Psychologist, 47(4), 259–280. doi:10.1080/00461520.2012.722511.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Alexander, P. A., White, C. S., & Daugherty, M. (1997). Analogical reasoning and early mathematics learning. In L. D. English (Ed.), Mathematical reasoning: analogies, metaphors, and images: studies in mathematical thinking and learning (pp. 117–147). Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
  5. Ahrens, K., Liu, H. L., Lee, C. Y., Gong, S. P., Fang, S. Y., & Hsu, Y. Y. (2007). Functional MRI of conventional and anomalous metaphors in Mandarin Chinese. Brain and Language, 100(2), 163–171. doi:10.1016/j.bandl.2005.10.004.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Angers, M. E. (2010). Mind, body, language, and “the embodied real” towards a psychoanalytically informed resolution of the antinomies of the Enlightenment. Issues in Psychoanalytic Psychology, 32(1–2), 73–105.Google Scholar
  7. Aubusson, P., Harrison, A. G., & Ritchie, S. (2006). Metaphor and analogy in science education. Dordrecht: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Baldo, J. V., Bunge, S. A., Wilson, S. M., & Dronkers, N. F. (2010). Is relational reasoning dependent on language? A voxel-based lesion symptom mapping study. Brain and Language, 113(2), 59–64. doi:10.1016/j.bandl.2010.01.004.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Ball, L. J., Hoyle, A. M., & Towse, A. S. (2010). The facilitatory effect of negative feedback on the emergence of analogical reasoning abilities. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 28(3), 583–602. doi:10.1348/026151009X461744.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Baillargeon, R., & Graber, M. (1987). Where’s the rabbit? 5.5-month-old infants’ representation of the height of a hidden object. Cognitive Development, 2(4), 375–392. doi:10.1016/S0885-2014(87)80014-X.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Baker, S. T., Friedman, O., & Leslie, A. M. (2010). The opposites task: using general rules to test cognitive flexibility in preschoolers. Journal of Cognition and Development, 11(2), 240–254. doi:10.1080/15248371003699944.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Bellocchi, A., & Ritchie, S. M. (2011). Investigating and theorizing discourse during analogy writing in chemistry. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 48(7), 771–792. doi:10.1002/tea.20428.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Bianchi, I., Savardi, U., & Burro, R. (2011a). Perceptual ratings of opposite spatial properties: do they lie on the same dimension? Acta Psychologica, 138(3), 405–418. doi:10.1016/j.actpsy.2011.08.003.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Bianchi, I., Savardi, U., & Kubovy, M. (2011b). Dimensions and their poles: a metric and topological approach to opposites. Language & Cognitive Processes, 26, 1232–1265. doi:10.1080/01690965.2010.520943.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Billow, R. M. (2003). Pursuing relational consciousness: thinking and antithinking in group. International Journal of Group Psychotherapy, 53(4), 459–477. doi:10.1521/ijgp.53.4.459.42835.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Binet, A., Simon, T., & Town, C. H. (1913). A method of measuring the development of the intelligence of young children. Chicago: Chicago Medical Book Company.Google Scholar
  17. Birney, D. P., & Halford, G. S. (2002). Cognitive complexity of suppositional reasoning: an application of the relational complexity metric to the knight-knave task. Thinking and Reasoning, 8(2), 109–134. doi:10.1080/13546780143000161.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Birney, D. P., Halford, G. S., & Andrews, G. (2006). Measuring the influence of complexity on relational reasoning: the development of the latin square task. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 66(1), 146–171. doi:10.1177/0013164405278570.
  19. Bohan, J., & Sanford, A. (2008). Semantic anomalies at the borderline of consciousness: an eye-tracking investigation. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 61(2), 232–239. doi:10.1080/17470210701617219.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Bohn, R. E., & Short, J. E. (2009). How much information? 2009 report on American consumers. La Jolla: UC San Diego Global Information Industry Center. Retrieved from http://hmi.ucsd.edu/pdf/HMI_2009_ConsumerReport_Dec9_2009.pd
  21. Bostrom, A. (2008). Lead is like mercury: risk comparisons, analogies and mental models. Journal of Risk Research, 11(1–2), 99–117. doi:10.1080/13669870701602956.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Braasch, J. L. G., & Goldman, S. R. (2010). The role of prior knowledge in learning from analogies in science texts. Discourse Processes, 47, 447–479. doi:10.1080/01638530903420960.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Broughton, S. H., & Sinatra, G. M. (2010). Text in the science classroom: promoting engagement to facilitate conceptual change. In M. G. McKeown & L. Kucan (Eds.), Bringing reading research to life (pp. 232–256). New York, NY US: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  24. Broughton, S. H., Sinatra, G. M., & Reynolds, R. E. (2010). The nature of the refutation text effect: an investigation of attention allocation. The Journal of Educational Research, 103(6), 407–423. doi:10.1080/00220670903383101.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Bruttin, C. D. (2011). Computerised assessment of an analogical reasoning test: effects of external memory strategies and their positive outcomes in young children and adolescents with intellectual disability. Educational and Child Psychology, Computerised Approaches to Assessment, 28(2), 18–32.Google Scholar
  26. Bulloch, M. J., & Opfer, J. E. (2009). What makes relational reasoning smart? Revisiting the perceptual-to-relational shift in the development of generalization. Developmental Science, 12(1), 114–122. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00738.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Bunge, S. A., Wendelken, C., Badre, D., & Wagner, A. D. (2005). Analogical reasoning and prefrontal cortex: evidence for separable retrieval and integration mechanisms. Cerebral Cortex, 15(3), 239–249. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhh126.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Cattell, R. B. (1940). A culture-free intelligence test. I. Journal of Educational Psychology, 31(3), 161–179. doi:10.1037/h0059043.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Chang, Y. J., Wang, F. T. Y., Chen, S. F., & Ma, T. S. (2012). Anomaly detection to increase commuter safety for individuals with cognitive impairments. Journal of Developmental and Physical Disabilities, 24(1), 9–17. doi:10.1007/s10882-011-9251-3.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Chase, W. G., & Simon, H. A. (1973). Perception in chess. Cognitive Psychology, 4(1), 55–81. doi:10.1016/0010-0285(73)90004-2.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Chen, C., Zhou, X., Chen, C., Dong, Q., Zang, Y., Qiao, S., Yang, T., et al. (2007). The neural basis of processing anomalous information. NeuroReport: For Rapid Communication of Neuroscience Research, 18(8), 747–751. doi:10.1097/WNR.0b013e3280ebb49b.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Chi, M. T. H., & Slotta, J. D. (1993). The ontological coherence of intuitive physics. Cognition and Instruction, 10(2–3), 249–260. doi:10.1207/s1532690xci1002&3_5.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Chi, M. T. H., & Roscoe, R. D. (2002). The processes and challenges of conceptual change. In M. Limon & L. Mason (Eds.), Reconsidering conceptual change: issues in theory and practice (pp. 3–27). Amsterdam: Kluwer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Chinn, C. A., & Anderson, R. C. (1998). The structure of discussions that promote reasoning. Teachers College Record: Topics for the New Educational Psychology, 100(2), 315–368.Google Scholar
  35. Chinn, C. A., & Brewer, W. F. (1993). The role of anomalous data in knowledge acquisition: a theoretical framework and implications for science instruction. Review of Educational Research, 63(1), 1–49. doi:10.2307/1170558.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Chinn, C. A., & Malhotra, B. A. (2002). Children’s responses to anomalous scientific data: how is conceptual change impeded? Journal of Educational Psychology, 94(2), 327–343. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.94.2.327.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Cho, S., Moody, T. D., Fernandino, L., Mumford, J. A., Poldrack, R. A., Cannon, T. D., Knowlton, B. J., et al. (2010). Common and dissociable prefrontal loci associated with component mechanisms of analogical reasoning. Cerebral Cortex, 20(3), 524–533. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhp121.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Cho, S., Holyoak, K. J., & Cannon, T. D. (2007). Analogical reasoning in working memory: resources shared among relational integration, interference resolution, and maintenance. Memory & Cognition, 35(6), 1445–1455. doi:10.3758/BF03193614.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Cole, M., & Wertschb, J. V. (1996). Beyond the individual-social antinomy in discussions of Piaget and Vygotsky. Human Development, 39(5), 250–256. doi:10.1159/000278475.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Crone, E. A., Wendelken, C., Van Leijenhorst, L., Honomichl, R. D., Christoff, K., & Bunge, S. A. (2009). Neurocognitive development of relational reasoning. Developmental Science, 12(1), 55–66. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00743.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Cubukcu, E., & Cetintahra, G. E. (2010). Does analogical reasoning with visual clues affect novice and experienced design students’ creativity? Creativity Research Journal, 22(3), 337–344. doi:10.1080/10400419.2010.504656.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Darden, L. (1995). Exemplars, abstractions, and anomalies: Representations and theory change in Mendelian and molecular genetics. In J. G. Lennox & G. Wolters (Eds.), Concepts, theories, and rationality in the biological sciences (pp. 137–158). Pittsburgh: University of Pittsburgh Press.Google Scholar
  43. Denaes, C. (2012). Analogical matrices in young children and students with intellectual disability: reasoning by analogy or reasoning by association? Journal of Applied Research in Intellectual Disabilities, 25(3), 271–281. doi:10.1111/j.1468-3148.2011.00665.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Depino, A. M., Lucchina, L., & Pitossi, F. (2011). Early and adult hippocampal TGF-β1 overexpression have opposite effects on behavior. Brain, Behavior, and Immunity, 25(8), 1582–1591. doi:10.1016/j.bbi.2011.05.007.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. De Saussure, F., (2011). Course in general linguistics (trans: Baskin, W.). New York: Columbia University Press. (Original work published 1916).Google Scholar
  46. Diakidoy, I. A. N., Mouskounti, T., & Ioannides, C. (2011). Comprehension and learning from refutation and expository texts. Reading Research Quarterly, 46(1), 22–38. doi:10.1598/RRQ.46.1.2.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Dumontheil, I., Houlton, R., Christoff, K., & Blakemore, S. J. (2010). Development of relational reasoning during adolescence. Developmental Science, 13(6), 15–24. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2010.01014.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Dunbar, K. (1995). How scientists really reason: scientific reasoning in real-world laboratories. In R. J. Sternberg & J. E. Davidson (Eds.), The nature of insight (pp. 365–395). Cambridge: MIT.Google Scholar
  49. Dunbar, K. (2001). The analogical paradox: why analogy is so easy in naturalistic settings yet so difficult in the psychological laboratory. In D. Gentner, K. J. Holyoak, & B. N. Kokinov (Eds.), The analogical mind: perspectives from cognitive science (pp. 313–334). Cambridge: MIT.Google Scholar
  50. Edwards, L., Figueras, B., Mellanby, J., & Langdon, D. (2011). Verbal and spatial analogical reasoning in deaf and hearing children: the role of grammar and vocabulary. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 16(2), 189–197. doi:10.1093/deafed/enq051.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Ehri, L. C., Satlow, E., & Gaskins, I. (2009). Grapho-phonemic enrichment strengthens keyword analogy instruction for struggling young readers. Reading & Writing Quarterly: Overcoming Learning Difficulties, 25(2–3), 162–191. doi:10.1080/10573560802683549.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. English, L. D. (1998). Children’s reasoning in solving relational problems of deduction. Thinking and Reasoning, 4(3), 249–281. doi:10.1080/135467898394157.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Eslinger, P. J., Blair, C., Wang, J., Lipovsky, B., Realmuto, J., Baker, D., Thorne, S., et al. (2009). Developmental shifts in fMRI activations during visuospatial relational reasoning. Brain and Cognition, 69(1), 1–10. doi:10.1016/j.bandc.2008.04.010.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Fales, C. L., Knowlton, B. I., Holyoak, K. J., Geschwind, D. H., Swerdloff, R. S., & Gonzalo, I. G. (2003). Working memory and relational reasoning in Klinefelter syndrome. Journal of International Neuropsychological Society, 9(6), 839–846. doi:10.1017/S1355617703960036.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Fast, K. V., & Campbell, G. (2004). “I still like Google:” university students’ perceptions of searching OPACs and the web. Proceedings of the American Society for Information Science and Technology, 41, 138–146.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. Farrington-Flint, L., Canobi, K. H., Woor, C., & Faulkner, D. (2007). The role of relational reasoning in children’s addition concepts. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 25, 227–246. doi:10.1348/026151006X108406.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  57. Farrington-Flint, L., & Wood, C. (2007). The role of lexical analogies in beginning reading: insights from children’s self-reports. Journal of Educational Psychology, 99, 326–338. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.99.2.326.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  58. Faustmann, A., Murdoch, B. E., Finnigan, S. P., & Copland, D. A. (2007). Effects of advancing age on the processing of semantic anomalies in adults: evidence from event-related brain potentials. Experimental Aging Research, 33(4), 439–460. doi:10.1080/03610730701525378.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  59. Ferguson, H. J., & Sanford, A. J. (2008). Anomalies in real and counterfactual worlds: an eye-movement investigation. Journal of Memory and Language, 58(3), 609–626. doi:10.1016/j.jml.2007.06.007.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  60. Fischer, R. S., Norberg, A., & Lundman, B. (2008). Embracing opposites: meanings of growing old as narrated by people aged 85. International Journal of Aging & Human Development, 67(3), 259–271. doi:10.2190/AG.67.3.d.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  61. Filik, R. (2008). Contextual override of pragmatic anomalies: evidence from eye movements. Cognition, 106(2), 1038–1046. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2007.04.006.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  62. Filik, R., & Leuthold, H. (2008). Processing local pragmatic anomalies in fictional contexts: evidence from the N400. Psychophysiology, 45(4), 554–558. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8986.2008.00656.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  63. Gardner, H. (1995). Perennial antinomies and perpetual redrawings: is there progress in the study of mind? In R. Solso & D. Massaro (Eds.), The science of the mind: 2001 and beyond (pp. 65–78). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  64. Gentner, D. (1983). Structure-mapping: a theoretical framework for analogy. Cognitive Science, 7(2), 155–170. doi:10.1207/s15516709cog0702_3.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  65. Gentner, D., Loewenstein, J., & Thompson, L. (2003). Learning and transfer: a general role for analogical encoding. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95(2), 393–405. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.95.2.393.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  66. Goel, V., & Dolan, R. J. (2001). Functional neuroanatomy of three-term relational reasoning. Neuropsychologia, 39(9), 901–909. doi:10.1016/S0028-3932(01)00024-0.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  67. Goldwater, M. B., Tomlinson, M. T., Echols, C. H., & Love, B. C. (2011). Structural priming as structure-mapping: children use analogies from previous utterances to guide sentence production. Cognitive Science, 35(1), 156–170. doi:10.1111/j.1551-6709.2010.01150.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  68. Gordon, P. C., & Moser, S. (2007). Insight into analogies: evidence from eye movements. Visual Cognition, 15(1), 20–35. doi:10.1080/13506280600871891.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  69. Goswami, U. (1992). Analogical reasoning in children. Hove: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
  70. Goswami, U., & Mead, F. (1992). Onset and rime awareness and analogies in reading. Reading Research Quarterly, 27(2), 152–162. doi:10.2307/747684.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  71. Goswami, U., & Bryant, P. (1992). Rhyme, analogy, and children’s reading. In P. B. Gough, L. C. Ehri, & R. Treiman (Eds.), Reading acquisition (pp. 49–63). Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
  72. Green, A. E., Fugelsang, J. A., Kraemer, D. J. M., & Dunbar, K. N. (2008). The micro-category account of analogy. Cognition, 106(2), 1004–1016. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2007.03.015.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  73. Green, A. E., Kraemer, D. J., Fugelsang, J. A., Gray, J. R., & Dunbar, K. N. (2012). Neural correlates of creativity in analogical reasoning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 38(2), 264–272. doi:10.1037/a0025764.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  74. Griffiths, J. R., & Brophy, P. (2005). Student searching behavior and the web: use of academic resources and Google. Library Trends, 53(4), 539–554.Google Scholar
  75. Heit, E., & Nicholson, S. P. (2010). The opposite of Republican: polarization and political categorization. Cognitive Science, 34(8), 1503–1516. doi:10.1111/j.1551-6709.2010.01138.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  76. Hesse, M. B. (1959). On defining analogy. Proceedings of the Aristotelian Society, 60, 79–100.Google Scholar
  77. Hofstadter, D. R. (2001). Epilogue: Analogy as the core of cognition. In D. Gentner, K. J. Holyoak, & B. N. Kokinov (Eds.), The analogical mind: perspectives from cognitive science (pp. 499–538). Cambridge: MIT.Google Scholar
  78. Holliway, D. R. (2007). Spontaneous analogies in referential writing. Communication & Cognition, 40(1–2), 127–158.Google Scholar
  79. Holyoak, K. J. (2012). Analogy and relational reasoning. In K. J. Holyoak & R. G. Morrison (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of thinking and reasoning. New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  80. Hummel, J. E., & Holyoak, K. J. (2005). Relational reasoning in a neurally plausible cognitive architecture: an overview of the LISA project. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 14(3), 153–157. doi:10.1111/j.0963-7214.2005.00350.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  81. Iozzi, L., & Barbieri, M. S. (2009). Preschoolers’ use of analogies in referential communication. First Language, 29(2), 192–207. doi:10.1177/0142723708099453.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  82. Ivanova, I., Pickering, M. J., Branigan, H. P., McLean, J. F., & Costa, A. (2012). The comprehension of anomalous sentences: evidence from structural priming. Cognition, 122(2), 193–209. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2011.10.013.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  83. James, W. (1890). The principles of psychology. New York: Henry Holt and Company.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  84. James, M. C., & Scharmann, L. C. (2007). Using analogies to improve the teaching performance of preservice teachers. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 44(4), 565–585. doi:10.1002/tea.20167.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  85. Kjeldergaard, P. M., & Higa, M. (1962). Degree of polarization and the recognition value of words selected from the semantic atlas. Psychological Reports, 11(3), 629–630. doi:10.2466/pr0.1962.11.3.629.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  86. Knowlton, B. J., Morrison, R. G., Hummel, J. E., & Holyoak, K. J. (2012). A neurocomputational system for relational reasoning. Trends in Cognitive Science, 16, 373–381. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2012.06.002.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  87. Koenig, C. S., Platt, R. D., & Griggs, R. A. (2007). Using dual-process theory and analogical transfer to explain facilitation on a hypothetico-deductive reasoning task. Psychological Research/Psychologische Forschung, 71(4), 495–502. doi:10.1007/s00426-006-0046-6.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  88. Kokinov, B., & Petrov, A. (2001). Integrating memory and reasoning in analogy-making: The AMBR model. In D. Gentner, K. J. Holyoak, & B. N. Kokinov (Eds.), The analogical mind: perspectives from cognitive science (pp. 499–538). Cambridge: MIT.Google Scholar
  89. Kostic, B., Cleary, A. M., Severin, K., & Miller, S. W. (2010). Detecting analogical resemblance without retrieving the source analogy. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 17(3), 405–411. doi:10.3758/PBR.17.3.405.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  90. Kosugi, D., Ishida, H., Murai, C., & Fujita, K. (2009). Nine- to 11-month-old infants’ reasoning about causality in anomalous human movements. Japanese Psychological Research, 51(4), 246–257. doi:10.1111/j.1468-5884.2009.00407.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  91. Krawczyk, D. C. (2012). The cognition and neuroscience of relational reasoning. Brain Research, 1428, 13–23. doi:10.1016/j.brainres.2010.11.080.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  92. Krawczyk, D. C., Morrison, R. G., Viskontas, I., Holyoak, K. J., Chow, T. W., Mendez, M. F., Miller, B. L., et al. (2008). Distraction during relational reasoning: the role of prefrontal cortex in interference control. Neuropsychologia, 46(7), 2020–2032. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.02.001.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  93. Krawczyk, D. C., McClelland, M. M., Donovan, C. M., Tillman, G. D., & Maguire, M. J. (2010). An fMRI investigation of cognitive stages in reasoning by analogy. Brain Research, 1342, 63–73. doi:10.1016/j.brainres.2010.04.039.
  94. Krawczyk, D. C., McClelland, M. M., & Donovan, C. M. (2011). A hierarchy for relational reasoning in the prefrontal cortex. Cortex: A Journal Devoted to the Study of the Nervous System and Behavior, 47(5), 588–597. doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2010.04.008.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  95. Kreezer, G., & Dallenbach, K. M. (1929). Learning the relation of opposition. The American Journal of Psychology, 41, 432–441. doi:10.2307/1414683.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  96. Kroger, J. K., Sabb, F. W., Fales, C. L., Bookheimer, S. Y., Cohen, M. S., & Holyoak, K. J. (2002). Recruitment of anterior dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in human reasoning: a parametric study of relational complexity. Cerebral Cortex, 12(5), 477–485. doi:10.1093/cercor/12.5.477.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  97. Klein, P. D., Piacente-Cimini, S., & Williams, L. A. (2007). The role of writing in learning from analogies. Learning and Instruction, 17(6), 595–611. doi:10.1016/j.learninstruc.2007.09.006.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  98. Kuhn, T. S. (1996). The structure of scientific revolutions. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  99. Kuhn, D., & Udell, W. (2007). Coordinating own and other perspectives in argument. Thinking and Reasoning, 13(2), 90–104. doi:10.1080/13546780600625447.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  100. Lam, W. K., Maxwell, J. P., & Masters, R. S. W. (2009). Analogy versus explicit learning of a modified basketball shooting task: performance and kinematic outcomes. Journal of Sports Sciences, 27(2), 179–191. doi:10.1080/02640410802448764.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  101. Lee, H. S., & Holyoak, K. J. (2008). The role of causal models in analogical inference. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 34(5), 1111–1122. doi:10.1037/a0012581.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  102. Leech, R., Mareschal, D., & Cooper, R. P. (2007). Relations as transformations: implications for analogical reasoning. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 60(7), 897–908. doi:10.1080/17470210701288599.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  103. Leroy, S., Parisse, C., & Maillart, C. (2012). Analogical reasoning in children with specific language impairment. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics, 26(4), 380–396. doi:10.3109/02699206.2011.641059.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  104. List, A., Grossnickle, E. M., & Alexander, P. A., (2012). Students’ source selections, justifications, and evaluations when responding to different question types. International Conference on Conceptual Change, Trier, Germany.Google Scholar
  105. Maguire, M. J., McClelland, M. M., Donovan, C. M., Tillman, G. D., & Krawczyk, D. C. (2012). Tracking cognitive phases in analogical reasoning with event-related potentials. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 38(2), 273–281. doi:10.1037/a0025485.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  106. Marková, I. (1987). On the interaction of opposites in psychological processes. Journal for the Theory of Social Behaviour, 17(3), 279–299. doi:10.1111/j.1468-5914.1987.tb00100.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  107. Martin, R. W. (1991). Examining personal relationship thinking: the relational cognition complexity instrument. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 8(4), 467–480. doi:10.1177/026540759184002.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  108. Mason, M. F., Magee, J. C., Kuwabara, K., & Nind, L. (2010). Specialization in relational reasoning: the efficiency, accuracy, and neural substrates of social versus nonsocial inferences. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 1(4), 318–326. doi:10.1177/1948550610366166.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  109. Matte-Blanco, I. (1988). Thinking, feeling, and being: clinical reflections on the fundamental antinomy of human beings and world. Florence: Routledge.Google Scholar
  110. Mitchell, M. (1993). Analogy-making as perception: a computer model. Cambridge: MIT.Google Scholar
  111. Mishra, R. K., Pandey, A., & Srinivasan, N. (2011). Revisiting the scrambling complexity hypothesis in sentence processing: a self-paced reading study on anomaly detection and scrambling in Hindi. Reading and Writing, 24(6), 709–727. doi:10.1007/s11145-010-9255-x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  112. Mosenthal, P. B. (1988). Anopheles and antinomies in reading research. The Reading Teacher, 42(3), 234–235.Google Scholar
  113. Morrison, R. G., Doumas, L. A. A., & Richland, L. E. (2011). A computational account of children’s analogical reasoning: balancing inhibitory control in working memory and relational representation. Developmental Science, 14(3), 516–529. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2010.00999.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  114. Morsanyi, K., & Holyoak, K. J. (2010). Analogical reasoning ability in autistic and typically developing children. Developmental Science, 13(4), 578–587. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00915.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  115. Munnelly, A., Dymond, S., & Hinton, E. C. (2010). Relational reasoning with derived comparative relations: a novel model of transitive inference. Behavioural Processes, 85(1), 8–17. doi:10.1016/j.beproc.2010.05.007.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  116. Mutonyi, H. (2007). Analogies, metaphors, and similes for HIV/AIDS among Ugandan grade 11 students. Alberta Journal of Educational Research, 53(2), 189–206.Google Scholar
  117. Opfer, J. E., & Gelman, S. A. (2011). Development of the animate–inanimate distinction. In U. Goswami (Ed.), The Wiley-Blackwell handbook of childhood cognitive development (2nd ed., pp. 213–238). Malden: Wiley-Blackwell.Google Scholar
  118. PBS Newshour Online (2010, December 10). Math, science, reading scores show U.S. schools slipping behind. Retrieved from http://www.pbs.org/newshour/extra/features/us/july-dec10/education_12-10.html
  119. Perret, P., Bailleux, C., & Dauvier, B. (2011). The influence of relational complexity and strategy selection on children’s reasoning in the Latin square task. Cognitive Development, 26(2), 127–141. doi:10.1016/j.cogdev.2010.12.003.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  120. Piaget, J. (1928/1966). Judgment and reasoning in the child. Totowa: Littlefield, Adams, & Co.Google Scholar
  121. Prado, J., Van der Henst, J. B., & Noveck, I. A. (2008). Spatial associations in relational reasoning: evidence for a SNARC-like effect. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 61(8), 1143–1150. doi:10.1080/17470210801954777.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  122. Prado, J., Van Der Henst, J. B., & Noveck, I. A. (2010). Recomposing a fragmented literature: how conditional and relational arguments engage different neural systems for deductive reasoning. NeuroImage, 51(3), 1213–1221. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.03.026.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  123. Prehn, K., Heekeren, H. R., & van der Meer, E. (2011). Influence of affective significance on different levels of processing using pupil dilation in an analogical reasoning task. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 79(2), 236–243. doi:10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2010.10.014.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  124. Preusse, F., van der Meer, E., Deshpande, G., Krueger, F., & Wartenburger, I. (2011). Fluid intelligence allows flexible recruitment of the parieto-frontal network in analogical reasoning. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 5(22), 1–14. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2011.00022.Google Scholar
  125. Raven, J. C. (1941). Standardization of progressive matrices, 1938. The British Journal of Medical Psychology, 19, 137–150. doi:10.1111/j.2044-8341.1941.tb00316.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  126. Reinhart, A. L., Haring, S. H., Levin, J. R., Patall, E. A., & Robinson, D. H. (2013). Models of not-so-good behavior: yet another way to squeeze causality and recommendations for practice out of correlational data. Journal of Educational Psychology, 105(1), 241–247. doi:10.1037/a0030368.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  127. Richland, L. E., Chan, T. K., Morrison, R. G., & Au, T. K. F. (2010). Young children’s analogical reasoning across cultures: similarities and differences. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 105(1–2), 146–153. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2009.08.003.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  128. Richland, L. E., & McDonough, I. M. (2010). Learning by analogy: discriminating between potential analogs. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 35(1), 28–43. doi:10.1016/j.cedpsych.2009.09.001.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  129. Richland, L. E., Zur, O., & Holyoak, K. J. (2007). Cognitive supports for analogies in the mathematics classroom. Science, 316(5828), 1128–1129. doi:10.1126/science.1142103.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  130. Ruiz, F. J., & Luciano, C. (2011). Cross-domain analogies as relating derived relations among two separate relational networks. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 95(3), 369–385. doi:10.1901/jeab.2011.95-369.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  131. Russell, B., & Lackey, D. (1973). Essays in analysis. New York: Allen & Unwin.Google Scholar
  132. Siddiqi, H. (2012). The relevance of thinking-by-analogy for investors’ willingness-to-pay: an experimental study. Journal of Economic Psychology, 33(1), 19–29. doi:10.1016/j.joep.2011.08.008.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  133. Sanford, A. J., Leuthold, H., Bohan, J., & Sanford, A. J. S. (2011). Anomalies at the borderline of awareness: an ERP study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23(3), 514–523. doi:10.1162/jocn.2009.21370.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  134. Savage, R. S., Deault, L., Daki, J., & Aouad, J. (2011). Orthographic analogies and early reading: evidence from a multiple clue word paradigm. Journal of Educational Psychology, 103, 190–205. doi:10.1037/a0021621.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  135. Schaeken, W., Van der Henst, J. B., Schroyens, W., & d’ Ydewalle, G. (2007). The mental models theory of relational reasoning: premises’ relevance, conclusions’ phrasing, and cognitive economy. In W. Schaeken, A. Vandierendonck, & W. Schroyens (Eds.), The mental models theory of reasoning: refinements and extensions (pp. 129–150). Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
  136. Schmidt, W. H., McKnight, C. C., Cogan, L. S., Jakwerth, P. M., & Houang, R. T. (1999). Facing the consequences: using TIMSS for a closer look at US mathematics and science education. New York: Kluwer Academic.Google Scholar
  137. Scruggs, T. E., Mastropieri, M. A., & Sullivan, G. S. (1994). Promoting relational thinking: elaborative interrogation for students with mild disabilities. Exceptional Children, 60(5), 450–457.Google Scholar
  138. Sebag, M., & Rouveirol, C. (2000). Resource-bounded relational reasoning: induction and deduction through stochastic matching. Machine Learning, Multistrategy Learning, 38(1), 41–62. doi:10.1023/A:1007629922420.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  139. Shaumyan, S. (2006). Antinomies of language and language operations of the mind. In H. R. Arabnia, E. B. Kozerenk, & S. Shaumyan (Eds.), Proceedings of the 2006 international conference on machine learning; models, technologies & applications, MLMTA (pp. 3–9). Las Vegas: CSREA.Google Scholar
  140. Schiff, R., & Ravid, D. (2007). Morphological analogies in Hebrew-speaking university students with dyslexia compared with typically developing gradeschoolers. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 36(3), 237–253. doi:10.1007/s10936-006-9043-6.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  141. Schmidt, G. L., Cardillo, E. R., Kranjec, A., Lehet, M., Widick, P., & Chatterjee, A. (2012). Not all analogies are created equal: associative and categorical analogy processing following brain damage. Neuropsychologia, 50(7), 1372–1379. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2012.02.022.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  142. Schulz, L. E., Goodman, N. D., Tenenbaum, J. B., & Jenkins, A. C. (2008). Going beyond the evidence: abstract laws and preschoolers’ responses to anomalous data. Cognition, 109(2), 211–223. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2008.07.017.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  143. Slotta, J. D., & Chi, M. T. H. (2006). Helping students understand challenging topics in science through ontology training. Cognition and Instruction, 24(2), 261–289. doi:10.1207/s1532690xci2402_3.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  144. Sinatra, G. M., & Broughton, S. H. (2011). Bridging reading comprehension and conceptual change in science education: the promise of refutation text. Reading Research Quarterly, 46(4), 374–393.Google Scholar
  145. Son, J. Y., Smith, L. B., & Goldstone, R. L. (2011). Connecting instances to promote children’s relational reasoning. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 108(2), 260–277. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2010.08.011.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  146. Sorensen, R. A. (2003). A brief history of the paradox: philosophy and the Labyrinths of the mind. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  147. Spearman, C. (1927). The abilities of man: their nature and measurement. New York: Macmillan.Google Scholar
  148. Stephens, A. C. (2006). Equivalence and relational thinking: preservice elementary teachers’ awareness of opportunities and misconceptions. Journal of Mathematics Teacher Education, 9(3), 249–278. doi:10.1007/s10857-006-9000-1.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  149. Sternberg, R. J. (1977). Intelligence, information processing, and analogical reasoning: the componential analysis of human abilities. Oxford: Lawrence Erlbaum Stevenson.Google Scholar
  150. Stevenson, C. E., Resing, W. C. M., & Froma, M. N. (2009). Analogical reasoning skill acquisition with self-explanation in 7–8 year olds: does feedback help? Educational and Child Psychology Reasoning in Children and Adolescents, 26(3), 6–17.Google Scholar
  151. Stevenson, C. E., Touw, K. W. J., & Resing, W. C. M. (2011). Computer or paper analogy puzzles: does assessment mode influence young children’s strategy progression? Educational and Child Psychology Computerised Approaches to Assessment, 28(2), 67–84.Google Scholar
  152. Stewart, I., Barnes-Holmes, D., Roche, B., & Smeets, P. M. (2001). Generating derived relational networks via the abstraction of common physical properties: a possible model of analogical reasoning. Psychological Record, 51(3), 381–408.Google Scholar
  153. Stewart, A. J., Kidd, E., & Haigh, M. (2009). Early sensitivity to discourse-level anomalies: evidence from self-paced reading. Discourse Processes, 46(1), 46–69. doi:10.1080/01638530802629091.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  154. Strømsø, H. I., & Bråten, I. (2010). The role of personal epistemology in the self-regulation of Internet-based learning. Metacognition and Learning, 5(1), 91–111.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  155. Summers, B., & Duxbury, D. (2012). Decision-dependent emotions and behavioral anomalies. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 118(2), 226–238. doi:10.1016/j.obhdp.2012.03.004.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  156. Tanca, M., Grossberg, S., & Pinna, B. (2010). Probing perceptual antinomies with the watercolor illusion and explaining how the brain resolves them. Seeing and Perceiving, 23(4), 295–333. doi:10.1163/187847510X532685.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  157. Taylor, E. G., & Hummel, J. E. (2009). Finding similarity in a model of relational reasoning. Cognitive Systems Research, Analogies—Integrating Cognitive Abilities, 10(3), 229–239. doi:10.1016/j.cogsys.2008.09.004.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  158. Thibaut, J. P., French, R., & Vezneva, M. (2010). Cognitive load and semantic analogies: searching semantic space. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 17(4), 569–574. doi:10.3758/PBR.17.4.569.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  159. Trey, L., & Khan, S. (2008). How science students can learn about unobservable phenomena using computer-based analogies. Computers in Education, 51(2), 519–529. doi:10.1016/j.compedu.2007.05.019.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  160. Trickett, S. B., Trafton, J. G., & Schunn, C. D. (2009). How do scientists respond to anomalies? Different strategies used in basic and applied science. Topics in Cognitive Science, 1, 711–729. doi:10.1111/j.1756-8765.2009.01036.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  161. Tunteler, E., & Resing, W. C. M. (2007). Effects of prior assistance in using analogies on young children’s unprompted analogical problem solving over time: a microgenetic study. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 77(1), 43–68. doi:10.1348/000709906X96923.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  162. Tunteler, E., & Resing, W. C. M. (2010). The effects of self and other scaffolding on progression and variation in children’s geometric analogy performance: a microgenetic research. Journal of Cognitive Education and Psychology, 9(3), 251–272. doi:10.1891/1945-8959.9.3.251.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  163. Tzuriel, D. (2007). Transfer effects of teaching conceptual versus perceptual analogies. Journal of Cognitive Education and Psychology, 6(2), 194–217. doi:10.1891/194589507787382232.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  164. Tzuriel, D., & George, T. (2009). Improvement of analogical reasoning and academic achievement by the Analogical Reasoning Programme (ARP). Educational and Child Psychology, Reasoning in Children and Adolescents, 26(3), 71–94.Google Scholar
  165. Tzuriel, D., & Shamir, A. (2010). Mediation strategies and cognitive modifiability in young children as a function of Peer Mediation with Young Children program and training in analogies versus math tasks. Journal of Cognitive Education and Psychology, 9(1), 48–72. doi:10.1891/1945-8959.9.1.48.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  166. Ullstadius, E., Carlstedt, B., & Gustafsson, J. E. (2008). The multidimensionality of verbal analogy items. International Journal of Testing, 8(2), 166–179. doi:10.1080/15305050802001243.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  167. Vakil, E., Lifshitz, H., Tzuriel, D., Weiss, I., & Arzuoan, Y. (2011). Analogies solving by individuals with and without intellectual disability: different cognitive patterns as indicated by eye movements. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 32(2), 846–856. doi:10.1016/j.ridd.2010.08.006.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  168. Van Gog, T., Paas, F., & Van Merriënboer, J. J. G. (2004). Process-oriented worked examples: improving transfer performance through enhanced understanding. Instructional Science, 32(1), 83–98. doi:10.1023/B:TRUC.0000021810.70784.b0.Google Scholar
  169. Van der Henst, J.-B., & Schaeken, W. (2005). The wording of conclusions in relational reasoning. Cognition, 97(1), 1–22. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2004.06.008.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  170. Van Tartwijk, J., Van Rijswijk, M., Tuithof, H., & Driessen, E. W. (2008). Using an analogy in the introduction of a portfolio. Teaching and Teacher Education, 24(4), 927–938. doi:10.1016/j.tate.2007.11.001.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  171. Vissers, C. T. W. M., Virgillito, D., Fitzgerald, D. A., Speckens, A. E. M., Tendolkar, I., van Oostrom, I., & Chwilla, D. J. (2010). The influence of mood on the processing of syntactic anomalies: evidence from P600. Neuropsychologia, 48(12), 3521–3531. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.08.001.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  172. Viskontas, I. V., Morrison, R. G., Holyoak, K. J., Hummel, J. E., & Knowlton, B. J. (2004). Relational integration, inhibition, and analogical reasoning in older adults. Psychology and Aging, 19(4), 581–591. doi:10.1037/0882-7974.19.4.581.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  173. Volle, E., Gilbert, S. J., Benoit, R. G., & Burgess, P. W. (2010). Specialization of the rostral prefrontal cortex for distinct analogy processes. Cerebral Cortex, 20(11), 2647–2659. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhq012.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  174. Walther, H. (2010). Anomalies in intertemporal choice, time-dependent uncertainty and expected utility—a common approach. Journal of Economic Psychology, 31(1), 114–130. doi:10.1016/j.joep.2009.11.006.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  175. Waltz, J. A., Knowlton, B. J., Holyoak, K. J., Boone, K. B., Mishkin, F. S., de Menezes Santos, M., Thomas, C. R., & Miller, B. L. (1999). A system for relational reasoning in human prefrontal cortex. Psychological Science, 10(2), 119–125. doi:10.1111/1467-9280.00118.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  176. Warren, R. E., Allen, K. V., Sommerfield, A. J., Deary, I. J., & Frier, B. M. (2004). Acute hypoglycemia impairs nonverbal intelligence. Diabetes Care, 27(6), 1447–1448.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  177. Watson, C. E., & Chatterjee, A. (2012). A bilateral frontoparietal network underlies visuospatial analogical reasoning. NeuroImage, 59(3), 2831–2838. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.09.030.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  178. Watts-Perotti, J., & Woods, D. D. (2009). Cooperative advocacy: an approach for integrating diverse perspectives in anomaly response. Computer Supported Cooperative Work, 18(2), 175–198. doi:10.1007/s10606-008-9085-4.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  179. Weber, K., & Lavric, A. (2008). Syntactic anomaly elicits a lexico-semantic (N400) ERP effect in the second language but not the first. Psychophysiology, 45(6), 920–925. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8986.2008.00691.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  180. Wendelken, C., Nakhabako, N., Donohue, S. E., Carter, C. S., & Bunge, S. A. (2008). “Brain is to thought as stomach is to??”: investigating the role of rostrolateral prefrontal cortex in relational reasoning. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20(4), 682–693. doi:10.1162/jocn.2008.20055.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  181. Wertheimer, M. (1900). Gestalt theory. Raleigh: Hayes Barton.Google Scholar
  182. White, C. S., & Caropreso, E. J. (1989). Training in analogical reasoning processes: effects on low socioeconomic status preschool children. The Journal of Educational Research, 83(2), 112–118.Google Scholar
  183. Zheng, R. Z., Yang, W., Garcia, D., & McCadden, E. P. (2008). Effects of multimedia and schema induced analogical reasoning on science learning. Journal of Computer Assisted Living, 24(6), 474–482. doi:10.1111/j.1365-2729.2008.00282.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  184. Zhao, M., Meng, H., Xu, Z., Du, F., Liu, T., Li, Y., & Chen, F. (2011). The neuromechanism underlying verbal analogical reasoning of metaphorical relations: an event-related potentials study. Brain Research, 1425, 62–74. doi:10.1016/j.brainres.2011.09.041.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media New York 2013

Authors and Affiliations

  • Denis Dumas
    • 1
  • Patricia A. Alexander
    • 1
  • Emily M. Grossnickle
    • 1
  1. 1.Department of Human Development and Quantitative MethodologyUniversity of MarylandCollege ParkUSA

Personalised recommendations