Advertisement

Cognitive Therapy and Research

, Volume 42, Issue 4, pp 447–460 | Cite as

The Brief State Rumination Inventory (BSRI): Validation and Psychometric Evaluation

  • Igor MarchettiEmail author
  • Nilly Mor
  • Carlo Chiorri
  • Ernst H. W. Koster
Original Article

Abstract

Depressive rumination is an emotion regulation strategy that is considered a major risk factor for depression and other emotional disorders. While well-established measures of trait rumination are available, a psychometrically sound measure of state rumination is lacking. We report on the development and validation of a new self-report measure, the Brief State Rumination Inventory (BSRI), in both Dutch and English. In Study 1, we report the results of a multi-group confirmatory factor analysis across three independent samples (n = 155; n = 141; n = 199). The analysis supported the unidimensionality and measurement invariance of the 8-item BSRI. We also examined its construct validity, showing that scores on the BSRI were positively related to measures of negative affect, trait rumination, and symptoms of depression and anxiety. Scores were negatively related to adaptive emotion regulation strategies and to positive affect. In Study 2 (n = 60), we demonstrated the measure’s sensitivity to an experimental manipulation of rumination. Taken together, these findings suggest that the BSRI is a quick-to-administer, valid, and reliable measure of state rumination.

Keywords

Rumination State measure Questionnaire BSRI 

Notes

Acknowledgements

This research was funded in part by the Research Foundation Flanders, which supports Igor Marchetti as postdoctoral research fellow (FWO14/PDO/115). Nilly Mor is support by the Israel Science Foundation (ISF 1519/13). The authors have no competing interests in this research. The authors thank Noa Avirbach, Tal Ganor, and Baruch Perlman for their assistance in the item development process.

Compliance with Ethical Standards

Conflict of Interest

Igor Marchetti, Nilly Mor, Carlo Chiorri, and Ernst H. W. Koster declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Informed Consent

All procedures followed were in accordance with the ethical standards of the responsible committee on human experimentation (national and institutional). Informed consent was obtained from all individual subjects participating in the study.

Animal Rights Statement

No animal studies were carried out by the authors for this article.

Supplementary material

10608_2018_9901_MOESM1_ESM.docx (184 kb)
Supplementary material 1 (DOCX 183 KB)

References

  1. Abend, R., Dan, O., Maoz, K., Raz, S., & Bar-Haim, Y. (2014). Reliability, validity and sensitivity of a computerized visual analog scale measuring state anxiety. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 45(4), 447–453.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbtep.2014.06.004.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  2. Asparouhov, T., & Muthén, B. (2009). Exploratory structural equation modeling. Structural Equation Modeling, 16, 397–438.  https://doi.org/10.1080/10705510903008204.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Augustine, A. A., & Larsen, R. J. (2012). Is a trait really the mean of states? Similarities and differences between traditional and aggregate assessments of personality. Journal of Individual Differences, 33(3), 131–137.  https://doi.org/10.1027/1614-0001/a000083.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Beck, A. T., Steer, R. A., & Brown, G. K. (1996). Manual for the beck depression inventory-II. San Antonio: The Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
  5. Benjamini, Y., & Hochberg, Y. (2000). On the adaptive control of the false discovery fate in multiple testing with independent statistics. Journal of Educational and Behavioral Statistics, 25(1), 60–83.  https://doi.org/10.3102/10769986025001060.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Bernblum, R., & Mor, N. (2010). Rumination and emotion-related biases in refreshing information. Emotion, 10(3), 423–432.  https://doi.org/10.1037/a0018427.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  7. Borkenau, P., & Ostendorf, F. (1998). The Big Five as states: How useful is the five-factor model to describe intraindividual variations over time? Journal of Research in Personality, 32(2), 202–221.  https://doi.org/10.1006/jrpe.1997.2206.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Brown, K. W., & Ryan, R. M. (2003). The benefits of being present: Mindfulness and its role in psychological well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84(4), 822–848.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.84.4.822.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  9. Byrne, B. M., Shavelson, R. J., & Muthen, B. (1989). Testing for the equivalence of factor covariance and mean structures—The issue of partial measurement invariance. Psychological Bulletin, 105(3), 456–466.  https://doi.org/10.1037//0033-2909.105.3.456.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Chambers, R., Lo, B. C. Y., & Allen, N. B. (2008). The impact of intensive mindfulness training on attentional control, cognitive style, and affect. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 32(3), 303–322.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s10608-007-9119-0.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Chen, F. F. (2007). Sensitivity of goodness of fit indexes to lack of measurement invariance. Structural Equation Modeling-A Multidisciplinary Journal, 14(3), 464–504.  https://doi.org/10.1080/10705510701301834.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Cheung, G. W., & Rensvold, R. B. (2001). The effects of model parsimony and sampling error on the fit of structural equation models. Organizational Research Methods, 4(3), 236–264.  https://doi.org/10.1177/109442810143004.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Ciesla, J. A., Reilly, L. C., Dickson, K. S., Emanuel, A. S., & Updegraff, J. A. (2012). Dispositional mindfulness moderates the effects of stress among adolescents: Rumination as a mediator. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 41(6), 760–770.  https://doi.org/10.1080/15374416.2012.698724.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  14. Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Earlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
  15. Connolly, S. L., & Alloy, L. B. (2017). Rumination interacts with life stress to predict depressive symptoms: An ecological momentary assessment study. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 97, 86–95.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2017.07.006.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  16. D’Avanzato, C., Joormann, J., Siemer, M., & Gotlib, I. H. (2013). Emotion regulation in depression and anxiety: Examining diagnostic specificity and stability of strategy use. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 37(5), 968–980.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s10608-013-9537-0.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. De Lissnyder, E., Derakshan, N., De Raedt, R., & Koster, E. H. W. (2011). Depressive symptoms and cognitive control in a mixed antisaccade task: Specific effects of depressive rumination. Cognition & Emotion, 25(5), 886–897.  https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2010.514711.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Du Troit, R., Pritchard, N., Heffernan, S., Simpson, T., & Fonn, D. (2002). A comparison of three different scales for rating contact lens handling. Optometry and Vision Science, 79(5), 313–320.  https://doi.org/10.1097/00006324-200205000-00011.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Ehring, T., Zetsche, U., Weidacker, K., Wahl, K., Schonfeld, S., & Ehlers, A. (2011). The Perseverative Thinking Questionnaire (PTQ): Validation of a content-independent measure of repetitive negative thinking. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 42(2), 225–232.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbtep.2010.12.003.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  20. Genet, J. J., & Siemer, M. (2012). Rumination moderates the effects of daily events on negative mood: Results from a diary study. Emotion, 12(6), 1329–1339.  https://doi.org/10.1037/a0028070.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  21. Gross, J. J. (1998). Antecedent- and response-focused emotion regulation: Divergent consequences for experience, expression, and physiology. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74(1), 224–237.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.74.1.224.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  22. Gross, J. J., & John, O. P. (2003). Individual differences in two emotion regulation processes: Implications for affect, relationships, and well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85(2), 348–362.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.85.2.348.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  23. Hau, K. T., & Marsh, H. W. (2004). The use of item parcels in structural equation modelling: Non-normal data and small sample sizes. British Journal of Mathematical & Statistical Psychology, 57, 327–351.  https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8317.2004.tb00142.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Hau, K. T., Marsh, H. W., & Wen, Z. (2004). Goodness of fit measures in structural equation modeling. International Journal of Psychology, 39(5–6), 513–513.Google Scholar
  25. Henry, J. D., & Crawford, J. R. (2005). The short-form version of the Depression Anxiety Stress Scales (DASS-21): Construct validity and normative data in a large non-clinical sample. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 44, 227–239.  https://doi.org/10.1348/014466505x29657.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  26. Hilt, L. M., Aldao, A., & Fischer, K. (2015). Rumination and multi-modal emotional reactivity. Cognition & Emotion, 29(8), 1486–1495.  https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2014.989816.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Horn, J. L. (1965). A rationale and test for the number of factors in factor-analysis. Psychometrika, 30(2), 179–185.  https://doi.org/10.1007/Bf02289447.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  28. Jöreskog, K. G. (1971). Simultaneous factor analysis in several populations. Psychometrika, 36(4), 409–426.  https://doi.org/10.1007/bf02291366.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Katz, B. A., Lustig, N., Assis, Y., & Yovel, I. (2017). Measuring regulation in the here and now: The development and validation of the state emotion regulation inventory (SERI). Psychological Assessment, 29(10), 1235–1248.  https://doi.org/10.1037/pas0000420.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  30. King, W. C., & Miles, E. W. (1995). A quasi-experimental assessment of the effect of computerizing noncognitive paper-and-pencil measurements: A test of measurement equivalence. Journal of Applied Psychology, 80(6), 643.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.80.6.643.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Kocovski, N. L., Endler, N. S., Rector, N. A., & Flett, G. L. (2005). Ruminative coping and post-event processing in social anxiety. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 43(8), 971–984.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2004.06.015.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  32. Koster, E. H. W., De Lissnyder, E., Derakshan, N., & De Raedt, R. (2011). Understanding depressive rumination from a cognitive science perspective: The impaired disengagement hypothesis. Clinical Psychology Review, 31(1), 138–145.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2010.08.005.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  33. Koster, E. H. W., Fang, L., Marchetti, I., Ebner-Priemer, U., Kirsch, P., Huffziger, S., & Kuehner, C. (2015). Examining the relation between mood and rumination in remitted depressed individuals: A dynamic systems analysis. Clinical Psychological Science, 3(4), 619–627.  https://doi.org/10.1177/2167702615578129.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Kuehner, C., & Weber, I. (1999). Responses to depression in unipolar depressed patients: An investigation of Nolen-Hoeksema’s response styles theory. Psychological Medicine, 29(6), 1323–1333.  https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291799001282.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  35. Kwon, P., & Olson, M. L. (2007). Rumination and depressive symptoms: Moderating role of defense style immaturity. Personality and Individual Differences, 43(4), 715–724.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2007.01.012.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. LeMoult, J., Arditte, K. A., D’Avanzato, C., & Joormann, J. (2013). State rumination: Associations with emotional stress reactivity and attention biases. Journal of Experimental Psychopathology, 4(5), 471–484.  https://doi.org/10.5127/jep.029112.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  37. Lyubomirsky, S., Layous, K., Chancellor, J., & Nelson, S. K. (2015). Thinking about rumination: The scholarly contributions and intellectual legacy of Susan Nolen-Hoeksema. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 11, 1–22.  https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-032814-112733.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  38. Lyubomirsky, S., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (1995). Effects of self-focused rumination on negative thinking and interpersonal problem-solving. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69(1), 176–190.  https://doi.org/10.1037//0022-3514.69.1.176.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  39. MacCallum, R. C., Wegener, D. T., Uchino, B. N., & Fabrigar, L. R. (1993). The problem of equivalent models in applications of covariance structure analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 114(1), 185–199.  https://doi.org/10.1037//0033-2909.114.1.185.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  40. Mahoney, A. E. J., McEvoy, P. M., & Moulds, M. L. (2012). Psychometric properties of the Repetitive Thinking Questionnaire in a clinical sample. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 26(2), 359–367.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.janxdis.2011.12.003.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  41. Marchetti, I., Koster, E. H. W., & De Raedt, R. (2013). Rest-related dynamics of risk and protective factors for depression: A behavioral study. Clinical Psychological Science, 1(4), 443–451.  https://doi.org/10.1177/2167702613489668.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Marchetti, I., Koster, E. H. W., Klinger, E., & Alloy, L. B. (2016). Spontaneous thought and vulnerability to mood disorders: The dark side of the wandering mind. Clinical Psychological Science, 4(5), 835–857.  https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0168612.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  43. Marsh, H. W., Hau, K. T., Balla, J. R., & Grayson, D. (1998). Is more ever too much? The number of indicators per factor in confirmatory factor analysis. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 33(2), 181–220.  https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327906mbr3302_1.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  44. Martin, L. L., & Tesser, A. (1996). Some ruminative thoughts. In R. S. Wyer (Ed.), Advances in social cognition (Vol. 9, pp. 1–48). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
  45. McCabe, K. O., & Fleeson, W. (2016). Are traits useful? Explaining trait manifestations as tools in the pursuit of goals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 110(2), 287–301.  https://doi.org/10.1037/a0039490.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  46. McCullough, M. E., Bono, G., & Root, L. M. (2007). Rumination, emotion, and forgiveness: Three longitudinal studies. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 92(3), 490–505.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.92.3.490.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  47. Meng, X. L., Rosenthal, R., & Rubin, D. B. (1992). Comparing correlated correlation-coefficients. Psychological Bulletin, 111(1), 172–175.  https://doi.org/10.1037//0033-2909.111.1.172.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Meredith, W. (1993). Measurement invariance, factor-analysis and factorial invariance. Psychometrika, 58(4), 525–543.  https://doi.org/10.1007/Bf02294825.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Moberly, N. J., & Watkins, E. R. (2008). Ruminative self-focus and negative affect: An experience sampling study. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 117(2), 314–323.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-843x.117.2.314.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  50. Molenaar, P. C. (2004). A manifesto on psychology as idiographic science: Bringing the person back into scientific psychology, this time forever. Measurement, 2(4), 201–218.  https://doi.org/10.1207/s15366359mea0204_1.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Mor, N., & Daches, S. (2015). Ruminative thinking: Lessons learned from cognitive training. Clinical Psychological Science, 3(4), 574–592.  https://doi.org/10.1177/2167702615578130.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Mor, N., Doane, L. D., Adam, E. K., Mineka, S., Zinbarg, R. E., Griffith, J. W., … Nazarian, M. (2010). Within-person variations in self-focused attention and negative affect in depression and anxiety: A diary study. Cognition & Emotion, 24(1), 48–62.  https://doi.org/10.1080/02699930802499715.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Mor, N., & Winquist, J. (2002). Self-focused attention and negative affect: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 128(4), 638–662.  https://doi.org/10.1037//0033-2909.128.4.638.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  54. Muthén, L. K., & Muthén, B. O. (1998–2012). Mplus user’s guide (6th ed.). Los Angelese: Muthén & Muthén.Google Scholar
  55. Muthen, L. K., & Muthen, B. O. (2002). How to use a Monte Carlo study to decide on sample size and determine power. Structural Equation Modeling, 9(4), 599–620.  https://doi.org/10.1207/S15328007sem0904_8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. Newby, J. M., & Moulds, M. L. (2012). A comparison of the content, themes, and features of intrusive memories and rumination in major depressive disorder. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 51(2), 197–205.  https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8260.2011.02020.x.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  57. Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (1991). Responses to depression and their effects on the duration of depressive episodes. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 100(4), 569–582.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-843x.100.4.569.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  58. Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2000). The role of rumination in depressive disorders and mixed anxiety/depressive symptoms. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 109(3), 504–511.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-843x.109.3.504.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  59. Nolen-Hoeksema, S., & Morrow, J. (1991). A prospective-study of depression and posttraumatic stress symptoms after a natural disaster - the 1989 Loma-Prieta Earthquake. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61(1), 115–121.  https://doi.org/10.1037//0022-3514.61.1.115.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  60. Nolen-Hoeksema, S., & Morrow, J. (1993). Effects of rumination and distraction on naturally-occurring depressed mood. Cognition & Emotion, 7(6), 561–570.  https://doi.org/10.1080/02699939308409206.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  61. Nolen-Hoeksema, S., Morrow, J., & Fredrickson, B. L. (1993). Response styles and the duration of episodes of depressed mood. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 102(1), 20–28.  https://doi.org/10.1037//0021-843x.102.1.20.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  62. Nolen-Hoeksema, S., Parker, L. E., & Larson, J. (1994). Ruminative coping with depressed mood following loss. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67(1), 92–104.  https://doi.org/10.1037//0022-3514.67.1.92.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  63. Nolen-Hoeksema, S., Stice, E., Wade, E., & Bohon, C. (2007). Reciprocal relations between rumination and bulimic, substance abuse, and depressive symptoms in female adolescents. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 116(1), 198–207.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-843x.116.1.198.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  64. Nolen-Hoeksema, S., & Watkins, E. R. (2011). A heuristic for developing transdiagnostic models of psychopathology: Explaining multifinality and divergent trajectories. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 6(6), 589–609.  https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691611419672.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  65. Nolen-Hoeksema, S., Wisco, B. E., & Lyubomirsky, S. (2008). Rethinking rumination. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 3(5), 400–424.  https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1745-6924.2008.00088.x.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  66. Olejnik, S., & Algina, J. (2003). Generalized eta and omega squared statistics: Measures of effect size for some common research designs. Psychological methods, 8(4), 434–447.  https://doi.org/10.1037/1082-989X.8.4.434.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  67. Peer, E., Brandimarte, L., Samat, S., & Acquisti, A. (2017). Beyond the Turk: Alternative platforms for crowdsourcing behavioral research. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 70, 153–163.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2017.01.006.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  68. Philippot, P., & Brutoux, F. (2008). Induced rumination dampens executive processes in dysphoric young adults. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 39(3), 219–227.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbtep.2007.07.001.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  69. Preacher, K. J., & Hayes, A. F. (2008). Asymptotic and resampling strategies for assessing and comparing indirect effects in multiple mediator models. Behavior Research Methods, 40(3), 879–891.  https://doi.org/10.3758/Brm.40.3.879.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  70. Roberts, H., Watkins, E. R., & Wills, A. J. (2013). Cueing an unresolved personal goal causes persistent ruminative self-focus: An experimental evaluation of control theories of rumination. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 44(4), 449–455.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbtep.2013.05.004.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  71. Robinson, M. D., & Clore, G. L. (2002). Episodic and semantic knowledge in emotional self-report: Evidence for two judgment processes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83(1), 198–215.  https://doi.org/10.1037//0022-3514.83.1.198.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  72. Rossi, V., & Pourtois, G. (2012). Transient state-dependent fluctuations in anxiety measured using STAI, POMS, PANAS or VAS: A comparative review. Anxiety Stress and Coping, 25(6), 603–645.  https://doi.org/10.1080/10615806.2011.582948.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  73. Ryan, J. M., Corry, J. R., Attewell, R., & Smithson, M. J. (2002). A comparison of an electronic version of the SF-36 General Health Questionnaire to the standard paper version. Quality of Life Research, 11(1), 19–26.  https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1014415709997.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  74. Samson, A. C., Kreibig, S. D., Soderstrom, B., Wade, A. A., & Gross, J. J. (2016). Eliciting positive, negative and mixed emotional states: A film library for affective scientists. Cognition & Emotion, 30(5), 827–856.  https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2015.1031089.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  75. Selcuk, E., Zayas, V., Gunaydin, G., Hazan, C., & Kress, E. (2012). Mental representations of attachment figures facilitate recovery following upsetting autobiographical memory recall. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 103(2), 362–378.  https://doi.org/10.1037/a0028125.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  76. Smith, J. M., & Alloy, L. B. (2009). A roadmap to rumination: A review of the definition, assessment, and conceptualization of this multifaceted construct. Clinical Psychology Review, 29(2), 116–128.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2008.10.003.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  77. Steinmetz, H. (2013). Analyzing observed composite differences across groups: Is partial measurement invariance enough? Methodology, 9(1), 1–12.  https://doi.org/10.1027/1614-2241/a000049.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  78. Stone, A. A., Shiffman, S., Schwartz, J. E., Broderick, J. E., & Hufford, M. R. (2002). Patient non-compliance with paper diaries. British Medical Journal, 324(7347), 1193–1194.  https://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.324.7347.1193.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  79. Takano, K., & Tanno, Y. (2011). Diurnal variation in rumination. Emotion, 11(5), 1046–1058.  https://doi.org/10.1037/a0022757.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  80. Tavakol, M., & Dennick, R. (2011). Making sense of Cronbach’s alpha. International Journal of Medical Education, 2, 53–55.  https://doi.org/10.5116/ijme.4dfb.8dfd.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  81. Trapnell, P. D., & Campbell, J. D. (1999). Private self-consciousness and the five-factor model of personality: Distinguishing rumination from reflection. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76(2), 284–304.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.76.2.284.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  82. Treynor, W., Gonzalez, R., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2003). Rumination reconsidered: A psychometric analysis. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 27(3), 247–259.  https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1023910315561.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  83. Vandenberg, R. J., & Lance, C. E. (2000). A review and synthesis of the measurement invariance literature: Suggestions, practices, and recommendations for organizational research. Organizational Research Methods, 3(1), 4–70.  https://doi.org/10.1177/109442810031002.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  84. Watkins, E., & Brown, R. G. (2002). Rumination and executive function in depression: An experimental study. Journal of Neurology Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 72(3), 400–402.  https://doi.org/10.1136/jnnp.72.3.400.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  85. Watkins, E., & Mason, A. (2002). Mood as input and rumination. Personality and Individual Differences, 32(4), 577–587.  https://doi.org/10.1016/s0191-8869(01)00058-7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  86. Watkins, E. R. (2008). Constructive and unconstructive repetitive thought. Psychological Bulletin, 134(2), 163–206.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.134.2.163.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  87. Watkins, E. R., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2014). A habit-goal framework of depressive rumination. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 123(1), 24–34.  https://doi.org/10.1037/a0035540.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  88. Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect—The Panas Scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54(6), 1063–1070.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.54.6.1063.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  89. Zoccola, P. M., Dickerson, S. S., & Lam, S. (2012). Eliciting and maintaining ruminative thought: The role of social-evaluative threat. Emotion, 12(4), 673–677.  https://doi.org/10.1037/a0027349.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC, part of Springer Nature 2018

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Ghent UniversityGhentBelgium
  2. 2.Hebrew University of JerusalemJerusalemIsrael
  3. 3.University of GenoaGenoaItaly

Personalised recommendations