Advertisement

Does Modification of Implicit Associations Regarding Contamination Affect Approach Behavior and Attentional Bias?

  • Christina Dusend
  • Laura M. S. De Putter
  • Ernst H. W. Koster
  • Fanny A. Dietel
  • Ulrike Buhlmann
Original Article

Abstract

Individuals with contamination concerns show aberrant interpretational and attentional processes. Yet, it is unclear whether threat-related associations play a causal role in anxiety symptoms and attentional bias. The objective of our study was to investigate if training implicit associations affects stress reactivity and attention in the context of contamination concerns. In a double-blind randomized design, we used a modified Implicit Associations Task (IAT) to train associations between contamination and danger in a non-clinical sample (N = 121). Dependent measures were a brief-IAT to assess changes in associations, contamination-related behavior approach tasks, and a spatial cueing task to measure attentional bias. Results show that training successfully modified implicit associations. However, there were no transfer effects on approach behavior or attention. Findings suggest that the modified IAT is a useful task for training implicit associations, but that transfer to other domains (attention and behavior) is limited. Limitations and future implications are discussed.

Keywords

Cognitive bias modification Implicit associations Attention Contamination Stressor task 

Notes

Acknowledgements

We thank Laura Brockhoff, Christina Martin, Ina Tuxhorn and Steffi Wenderlich for their assistance in data collection and preparation. We thank Celeste Brennecka for her valuable support with language and style.

Compliance with Ethical Standards

Conflict of Interest

Christina Dusend, Laura M. S. de Putter, Ernst H. W. Koster, Fanny A. Dietel, and Ulrike Buhlmann declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Ethical Approval

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.

Research Involving Human and Animal Participants

This article does not contain any studies with animals performed by any of the authors.

Informed Consent

Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.

Supplementary material

10608_2018_9991_MOESM1_ESM.docx (256 kb)
Supplementary material 1 (DOCX 257 KB)

References

  1. Abramowitz, J. S., Fabricant, L. E., Taylor, S., Deacon, B. J., McKay, D., & Storch, E. A. (2014). The relevance of analogue studies for understanding obsessions and compulsions. Clinical Psychology Review, 34(3), 206–217.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2014.01.004.Google Scholar
  2. Amir, N., Bomyea, J., & Beard, C. (2010). The effect of single-session interpretation modification on attention bias in socially anxious individuals. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 24(2), 178–182.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.janxdis.2009.10.005.Google Scholar
  3. Amir, N., Elias, J., Klumpp, H., & Przeworski, A. (2003). Attentional bias to threat in social phobia: Facilitated processing of threat or difficulty disengaging attention from threat? Behaviour Research and Therapy, 41(11), 1325–1335.Google Scholar
  4. Arjona, A., Escudero, M., & Gómez, C. M. (2016). Cue validity probability influences neural processing of targets. Biological Psychology, 119, 171–183.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2016.07.001.Google Scholar
  5. Beadel, J. R., Smyth, F. L., & Teachman, B. A. (2014). Change processes during cognitive bias modification for obsessive compulsive beliefs. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 38(2), 103–119.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s10608-013-9576-6.Google Scholar
  6. Beck, A. T., & Clark, D. A. (1997). An information processing model of anxiety: Automatic and strategic processes. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 35(1), 49–58.Google Scholar
  7. Beck, A. T., Steer, R. A., & Brown, G. K. (1996). BDI-II, Beck depression inventory: Manual (2nd edn.). San Antonio: Psychological Corp.; Harcourt Brace.Google Scholar
  8. Bowler, J. O., Hoppitt, L., Illingworth, J., Dalgleish, T., Ononaiye, M., Perez-Olivas, G., & Mackintosh, B. (2017). Asymmetrical transfer effects of cognitive bias modification: Modifying attention to threat influences interpretation of emotional ambiguity, but not vice versa. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 54, 239–246.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbtep.2016.08.011.Google Scholar
  9. Buchner, A., Erdfelder, E., Faul, F., & Lang, A.-G. (2011). G*Power (Version 3.1.3). Kiel.Google Scholar
  10. Cisler, J. M., & Olatunji, B. O. (2010). Components of attentional biases in contamination fear: Evidence for difficulty in disengagement. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 48(1), 74–78.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2009.09.003.Google Scholar
  11. Clerkin, E. M., & Teachman, B. A. (2010). Training implicit social anxiety associations: An experimental intervention. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 24(3), 300–308.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.janxdis.2010.01.001.Google Scholar
  12. Clerkin, E. M., & Teachman, B. A. (2011). Training interpretation biases among individuals with symptoms of obsessive compulsive disorder. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 42(3), 337–343.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbtep.2011.01.003.Google Scholar
  13. Conley, S. L., & Wu, K. D. (2018). Experimental modification of dysfunctional interpretations in individuals with contamination concerns. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 59, 56–64.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbtep.2017.11.005.Google Scholar
  14. Cristea, I. A., Mogoașe, C., David, D., & Cuijpers, P. (2015). Practitioner review: Cognitive bias modification for mental health problems in children and adolescents: A meta-analysis. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 56(7), 723–734.  https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.12383.Google Scholar
  15. Davis, M. L., Rosenfield, D., Bernstein, A., Zvielli, A., Reinecke, A., Beevers, C. G., et al. (2016). Attention bias dynamics and symptom severity during and following CBT for social anxiety disorder. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 84(9), 795–802.  https://doi.org/10.1037/ccp0000125.Google Scholar
  16. De Putter, L. M. S., Van Yper, L., & Koster, E. H. W. (2017). Obsessions and compulsions in the lab: A meta-analysis of procedures to induce symptoms of obsessive-compulsive disorder. Clinical Psychology Review, 52, 137–147.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2017.01.001.Google Scholar
  17. Dusend, C., De Putter, L. M. S., Koster, E. H. W., Möllmann, A., & Buhlmann, U. (2018). Inducing attentional bias in individuals with elevated contamination concerns: Effects on information processing and approach behavior (Manuscript in preparation).Google Scholar
  18. Ebert, I. D., Steffens, M. C., von Stülpnagel, R., & Jelenec, P. (2009). How to like yourself better, or chocolate less: Changing implicit attitudes with one IAT task. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 45(5), 1098–1104.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2009.06.008.Google Scholar
  19. Foa, E. B., Huppert, J. D., Leiberg, S., Langner, R., Kichic, R., Hajcak, G., & Salkovskis, P. M. (2002). The obsessive-compulsive inventory: Development and validation of a short version. Psychological Assessment, 14(4), 485–496.  https://doi.org/10.1037/1040-3590.14.4.485.Google Scholar
  20. Fox, E., Russo, R., & Dutton, K. (2002). Attentional bias for threat: Evidence for delayed disengagement from emotional faces. Cognition and Emotion, 16(3), 355–379.  https://doi.org/10.1080/02699930143000527.Google Scholar
  21. Gönner, S., Leonhart, R., & Ecker, W. (2007). Das Zwangsinventar OCI-R - die deutsche Version des Obsessive-Compulsive Inventory-Revised. PPmP: Psychotherapie · Psychosomatik · Medizinische Psychologie, 57(09/10), 395–404.  https://doi.org/10.1055/s-2007-970894.Google Scholar
  22. Green, J. S., & Teachman, B. A. (2012). Is “cootie” in the eye of the beholder? An experimental attempt to modify implicit associations tied to contamination fears. Journal of Experimental Psychopathology, 3(3), jep.026111.  https://doi.org/10.5127/jep.026111.Google Scholar
  23. Green, J. S., & Teachman, B. A. (2013). Predictive validity of explicit and implicit threat overestimation in contamination fear. Journal of Obsessive-Compulsive and Related Disorders, 2(1), 1–8.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jocrd.2012.09.002.Google Scholar
  24. Greenwald, A. G., McGhee, D. E., & Schwartz, J. L. (1998). Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: The implicit association test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74(6), 1464–1480.Google Scholar
  25. Greenwald, A. G., Nosek, B. A., & Banaji, M. R. (2003). Understanding and using the implicit association test: I. An improved scoring algorithm. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85(2), 197–216.Google Scholar
  26. Hautzinger, M., Keller, F., & Kühner, C. (2006). Beck Depressionsinventar: BDI II. Revision. Frankfurt/Main: Harcourt Test Services.Google Scholar
  27. Hezel, D. M., & McNally, R. J. (2016). A theoretical review of cognitive biases and deficits in obsessive–compulsive disorder. Biological Psychology, 121, 221–232.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2015.10.012.Google Scholar
  28. Inquisit 4 [Computer Software]. (2016). Retrieved from http://www.millisecond.com/download/.
  29. Jelinek, L., Hottenrott, B., & Moritz, S. (2009). When cancer is associated with illness but no longer with animal or zodiac sign: Investigation of biased semantic networks in obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD). Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 23(8), 1031–1036.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.janxdis.2009.07.003.Google Scholar
  30. Koster, E. H. W., De Raedt, R., Goeleven, E., Franck, E., & Crombez, G. (2005). Mood-congruent attentional bias in dysphoria: Maintained attention to and impaired disengagement from negative information. Emotion, 5(4), 446–455.  https://doi.org/10.1037/1528-3542.5.4.446.Google Scholar
  31. MacLeod, C., & Grafton, B. (2016). Anxiety-linked attentional bias and its modification: Illustrating the importance of distinguishing processes and procedures in experimental psychopathology research. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 86, 68–86.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2016.07.005.Google Scholar
  32. Menne-Lothmann, C., Viechtbauer, W., Höhn, P., Kasanova, Z., Haller, S. P., Drukker, M., et al. (2014). How to boost positive interpretations? A meta-analysis of the effectiveness of cognitive bias modification for interpretation. PLoS ONE, 9(6), e100925.  https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0100925.Google Scholar
  33. Mogg, K., & Bradley, B. P. (2018). Anxiety and threat-related attention: Cognitive-motivational framework and treatment. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 22(3), 225–240.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2018.01.001.Google Scholar
  34. Mogg, K., Holmes, A., Garner, M., & Bradley, B. P. (2008). Effects of threat cues on attentional shifting, disengagement and response slowing in anxious individuals. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 46(5), 656–667.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2008.02.011.Google Scholar
  35. Najmi, S., & Amir, N. (2010). The effect of attention training on a behavioral test of contamination fears in individuals with subclinical obsessive-compulsive symptoms. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 119(1), 136–142.  https://doi.org/10.1037/a0017549.Google Scholar
  36. Nicholson, E., Dempsey, K., & Barnes-Holmes, D. (2014). The role of responsibility and threat appraisals in contamination fear and obsessive-compulsive tendencies at the implicit level. Journal of Contextual Behavioral Science, 3(1), 31–37.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcbs.2013.11.001.Google Scholar
  37. Nosek, B. A., Bar-Anan, Y., Sriram, N., Axt, J., & Greenwald, A. G. (2014). Understanding and using the Brief Implicit Association Test: Recommended scoring procedures. PLoS ONE, 9(12), e110938.  https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0110938.Google Scholar
  38. Obsessive Compulsive Cognitions Working Group. (2005). Psychometric validation of the obsessive belief questionnaire and interpretation of intrusions inventory—Part 2: Factor analyses and testing of a brief version. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 43(11), 1527–1542.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2004.07.010.Google Scholar
  39. Ouimet, A. J., Gawronski, B., & Dozois, D. J. A. (2009). Cognitive vulnerability to anxiety: A review and an integrative model. Clinical Psychology Review, 29(6), 459–470.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2009.05.004.Google Scholar
  40. Park, J. W., Yoon, S. O., Kim, K. H., & Wyer, R. S. (2001). Effects of priming a bipolar attribute concept on dimension versus concept-specific accessibility of semantic memory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81(3), 405–420.Google Scholar
  41. Posner, M. I. (1980). Orienting of attention. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 32(1), 3–25.  https://doi.org/10.1080/00335558008248231.Google Scholar
  42. Salkovskis, P. M. (1985). Obsessional-compulsive problems: A cognitive-behavioural analysis. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 23(5), 571–583.  https://doi.org/10.1016/0005-7967(85)90105-6.Google Scholar
  43. Spielberger, C. D., Gorsuch, R. L., Lushene, R., Vagg, P. R., & Jacobs, G. A. (1977). State-trait anxiety inventory for adults. Redwood City: Mindgarden Inc.Google Scholar
  44. Sriram, N., & Greenwald, A. G. (2009). The brief implicit association test. Experimental Psychology, 56(4), 283–294.  https://doi.org/10.1027/1618-3169.56.4.283.Google Scholar
  45. Taylor, S., Coles, M. E., Abramowitz, J. S., Wu, K. D., Olatunji, B. O., Timpano, K. R., et al. (2010). How are dysfunctional beliefs related to obsessive-compulsive symptoms? Journal of Cognitive Psychotherapy, 24(3), 165–176.  https://doi.org/10.1891/0889-8391.24.3.165.Google Scholar
  46. Van Bockstaele, B., Verschuere, B., Tibboel, H., De Houwer, J., Crombez, G., & Koster, E. H. W. (2014). A review of current evidence for the causal impact of attentional bias on fear and anxiety. Psychological Bulletin, 140(3), 682–721.  https://doi.org/10.1037/a0034834.Google Scholar
  47. Võ, M. L. H., Conrad, M., Kuchinke, L., Urton, K., Hofmann, M. J., & Jacobs, A. M. (2009). The Berlin Affective Word List Reloaded (BAWL-R). Behavior Research Methods, 41(2), 534–538.  https://doi.org/10.3758/BRM.41.2.534.Google Scholar
  48. Williams, A. D., & Grisham, J. R. (2013). Cognitive Bias Modification (CBM) of obsessive compulsive beliefs. BMC Psychiatry.  https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-244X-13-256.Google Scholar
  49. Zvielli, A., Bernstein, A., & Koster, E. H. W. (2014). Dynamics of attentional bias to threat in anxious adults: Bias towards and/or away? PLoS ONE, 9(8), e104025.  https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0104025.Google Scholar
  50. Zvielli, A., Bernstein, A., & Koster, E. H. W. (2015). Temporal dynamics of attentional bias. Clinical Psychological Science, 3(5), 772–788.  https://doi.org/10.1177/2167702614551572.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC, part of Springer Nature 2019

Authors and Affiliations

  • Christina Dusend
    • 1
  • Laura M. S. De Putter
    • 2
  • Ernst H. W. Koster
    • 2
  • Fanny A. Dietel
    • 1
  • Ulrike Buhlmann
    • 1
  1. 1.Department of Clinical Psychology and PsychotherapyWestfälische Wilhelms-Universität MünsterMünsterGermany
  2. 2.Department of Experimental and Clinical Health PsychologyGhent UniversityGhentBelgium

Personalised recommendations