Skip to main content
Log in

Insomnia Symptom Severity Modulates The Impact of Sleep Deprivation on Attentional Biases to Emotional Information

  • Original Article
  • Published:
Cognitive Therapy and Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The present study evaluated the effect of experimental sleep deprivation on attentional biases to emotional information among a sample of 40 healthy, young adults. Participants were randomized into either a total sleep deprivation (i.e., 28 consecutive hours awake) or sleep control (i.e., 8-h sleep opportunity) condition. Participants also completed a modified version of the Dot Probe Task to assess attentional biases to positive and negative information and the insomnia severity index (ISI) to assess current insomnia symptom severity. While controlling for ISI scores, acute sleep deprivation was not associated with a greater bias to negative stimuli. In contrast, sleep deprivation predicted a significantly reduced bias to positive stimuli, but only among participants with relative low ISI scores. The present findings suggest that young adults with low levels of insomnia symptoms are particularly susceptible to the effects of sleep deprivation; such that acute sleep loss can reduce their natural tendencies to attend to positive information in the environment.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Agnew, H. W., Webb, W. B., & Williams, R. L. (1966). The first night effect: an EEG study of sleep. Psychophysiology, 2(3), 263–266. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8986.1966.tb02650.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Aiken, L., West, S., & Reno, R. (1991). Multiple regression: Testing and interpreting interactions. Newbury Park: Sage Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bar-Haim, Y., Lamy, D., Lee, P., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., & van IJzendoorn, M. H. (2007). Threat-related attentional bias in anxious and nonanxious individuals: A meta-analytic study. Psychological Bulletin, 133(1), 1–24.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bastien, C. H., Vallières, A., & Morin, C. M. (2001). Validation of the insomnia severity index as an outcome measure for insomnia research. Sleep Medicine, 2(4), 297–307. doi:10.1016/S1389-9457(00)00065-4.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Buysse, D. J., Reynolds, C. F., Monk, T. H., Berman, S. R., & Kupfer, D. J. (1989). The Pittsburgh sleep quality index: A new instrument for psychiatric practice and research. Psychiatry Research, 28(2), 193–213.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Carney, C. E., Buysse, D. J., Ancoli-Israel, S., Edinger, J. D., Krystal, A. D., Lichstein, K. L., & Morin, C. M. (2012). The consensus sleep diary: Standardizing prospective sleep self-monitoring. Sleep, 35(2), 287–302. doi:10.5665/sleep.1642.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Cisler, J. M., & Koster, E. H. W. (2010). Mechanisms of attentional biases towards threat in anxiety disorders: An integrative review. Clinical Psychology Review, 30(2), 203–216. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2009.11.003.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, S., Kamarck, T., & Mermelstein, R. (1983). A global measure of perceived stress. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 24(4), 385–396.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dawson, J. F., & Richter, A. W. (2006). Probing three-way interactions in moderated multiple regression: Development and application of a slope difference test. The Journal of Applied Psychology, 91(4), 917–926. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.91.4.917.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dohrenwend, B., Krassnoff, L., & Askenasy, A. (1978). Exemplification of a method for scaling life events: The PERI Life Events Scale. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 19(2), 205–229.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Espie, C. A. (2002). Insomnia: Conceptual issues in the development, persistence, and treatment of sleep disorder in adults. Annual Review of Psychology, 53, 215–243. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.53.100901.135243.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Espie, C. A., Broomfield, N. M., MacMahon, K. M. A., Macphee, L. M., & Taylor, L. M. (2006). The attention-intention-effort pathway in the development of psychophysiologic insomnia: A theoretical review. Sleep Medicine Reviews, 10(4), 215–245. doi:10.1016/j.smrv.2006.03.002.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fernandez-Mendoza, J., Calhoun, S., Bixler, E. O., Pejovic, S., Karataraki, M., Liao, D., & Vgontzas, A. N. (2010). Insomnia with objective short sleep duration is associated with deficits in neuropsychological performance: A general population study. Sleep, 33(4), 459–465. doi:10.1093/sleep/33.4.459.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Fortier-Brochu, E., Beaulieu-Bonneau, S., Ivers, H., & Morin, C. M. (2012). Insomnia and daytime cognitive performance: A meta-analysis. Sleep Medicine Reviews, 16(1), 83–94. doi:10.1016/j.smrv.2011.03.008.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Franzen, P. L., Buysse, D. J., Dahl, R. E., Thompson, W., & Siegle, G. J. (2009). Sleep deprivation alters pupillary reactivity to emotional stimuli in healthy young adults. Biological Psychology, 80(3), 300–305. doi:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2008.10.010.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Giedke, H., & Schwäzler, F. (2002). Therapeutic use of sleep deprivation in depression. Sleep Medicine Reviews, 6(5), 361–377. doi:10.1016/S1087-0792(02)90235-2.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gotlib, I., & Neubauer, D. (2000). Information-processing approaches to the study of cognitive biases in depression. In S. Johnson, A. Hayes, Ti. Field, N. Schneiderman & P. McCabe (Eds.), Stress, coping, and depression (pp. 117–142). Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gotlib, I. H., Krasnoperova, E., Neubauer Yue, D., & Joormann, J. (2004). Attentional biases for negative interpersonal stimuli in clinical depression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 113(1), 127–135. doi:10.1037/0021-843X.113.1.127.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gujar, N., Yoo, S.-S., Hu, P., & Walker, M. (2011). Sleep deprivation amplifies reactivity of brain reward networks, biasing the appraisal of positive emotional experiences. The Journal of Neuroscience, 31(12), 4466–4474. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3220-10.2011.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Haeffel, G. J., Thiessen, E. D., Campbell, M. W., Kaschak, M. P., & McNeil, N. M. (2009). Theory, not cultural context, will advance American psychology. The American Psychologist, 64(6), 570–571–574. doi:10.1037/a0016191.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Harris, K., Spiegelhalder, K., Espie, C. A., MacMahon, K. M. A., Woods, H. C., & Kyle, S. D. (2015). Sleep-related attentional bias in insomnia: A state-of-the-science review. Clinical Psychology Review, 42, 16–27. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2015.08.001.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Harvey, A. (2002). A cognitive model of insomnia. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 40(8), 869–893. doi:10.1016/S0005-7967(01)00061-4.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Horne, J. A., & Ostberg, O. (1975). A self-assessment questionnaire to determine morningness-eveningness in human circadian rhythms. International Journal of Chronobiology, 4(2), 97–110.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jansson-Fröjmark, M., Bermås, M., & Kjellén, A. (2013). Attentional bias in insomnia: The dot-probe task with pictorial stimuli depicting daytime fatigue/malaise. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 37(3), 534–546. doi:10.1007/s10608-012-9486-z.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Johns, M. (1991). A new method for measuring daytime sleepiness: The Epworth sleepiness scale. Sleep, 14(6), 540–545.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Joormann, J., & Gotlib, I. H. (2007). Selective attention to emotional faces following recovery from depression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 116(1), 80–85. doi:10.1037/0021-843X.116.1.80.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Joormann, J., Talbot, L., & Gotlib, I. H. (2007). Biased processing of emotional information in girls at risk for depression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 116(1), 135–143. doi:10.1037/0021-843X.116.1.135.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kroenke, K., Spitzer, R. L., & Williams, J. B. W. W. (2001). The PHQ-9: Validity of a brief depression severity measure. Journal of General Internal Medicine, 16(9), 605–613. doi:10.1046/j.1525-1497.2001.016009606.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • MacLeod, C. M., Mathews, A., & Tata, P. (1986). Attentional bias in emotional disorders. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 95(1), 15–20. doi:10.1037/0021-843X.95.1.15.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • MacMahon, K. M. A., Broomfield, N. M., & Espie, C. A. (2006). Attention bias for sleep-related stimuli in primary insomnia and delayed sleep phase syndrome using the dot-probe task. Sleep, 29(11), 1420–1427.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mai, E., & Buysse, D. J. (2008). Insomnia: Prevalence, impact, pathogenesis, differential diagnosis, and evaluation. Sleep Medicine Clinics, 3(2), 167–174. doi:10.1016/j.jsmc.2008.02.001.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Mcglinchey, E., Talbot, S., Chang, K., Kaplan, K. A., Dahl, R. E., & Harvey, A. G. (2011). The effect of sleep deprivation on vocal expression of emotion in adolescents and adults. Sleep, 34(9), 1233–1241. doi:10.5665/SlEEp.1246.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Mogg, K., & Bradley, B. P. (2005). Attentional bias in generalized anxiety disorder versus depressive disorder. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 29(1), 29–45. doi:10.1007/s10608-005-1646-y.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Morin, C. M. (1993). Insomnia: Psychological assessment and management. New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Morin, C. M., & Jarrin, D. C. (2013). Epidemiology of insomnia. Sleep Medicine Clinics, 8(3), 281–297. doi:10.1016/j.jsmc.2013.05.002.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ohayon, M. M. (2002). Epidemiology of insomnia: What we know and what we still need to learn. Sleep Medicine Reviews, 6(2), 97–111. doi:10.1053/smrv.2002.0186.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Riedel, B. W., & Lichstein, K. L. (2000). Insomnia and daytime functioning. Sleep Medicine Reviews, 4(3), 277–298. doi:10.1053/smrv.1999.0074.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rogosa, D. (1980). Comparing nonparallel regression lines. Psychological Bulletin, 88(2), 307.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sadeh, A., Sharkey, K., & Carskadon, M. (1994). Activity-based sleep—wake identification: An empirical test of methodological issues. Sleep, 17(3), 201–207.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Spiegelhalder, K., Espie, C., & Riemann, D. (2009). Is sleep-related attentional bias due to sleepiness or sleeplessness? Cognition and Emotion, 23, 541–550. doi:10.1080/02699930802022129.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Spiegelhalder, K., Kyle, S. D., Feige, B., Prem, M., Nissen, C., Espie, C., & Riemann, D. (2010). The impact of sleep-related attentional bias on polysomnographically measured sleep in primary insomnia. Sleep, 33(1), 107–112.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Spitzer, R. L., Kroenke, K., Williams, J. B. W., & Löwe, B. (2006). A brief measure for assessing generalized anxiety disorder: The GAD-7. Archives of Internal Medicine, 166(10), 1092–1097.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Talbot, L. S., McGlinchey, E. L., Kaplan, K. A., Dahl, R. E., & Harvey, A. G. (2010). Sleep deprivation in adolescents and adults: Changes in affect. Emotion (Washington, D. C.), 10(6), 831–841. doi:10.1037/a0020138.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tottenham, N., Borscheid, A., Ellertsen, K., Marcus, D., & Nelson, C. A. (2002). The NimStim face set. Retreived from http://www.macbrain.org/faces/index.htm.

  • Vgontzas, A. N., Liao, D., Pejovic, S., Calhoun, S., Karataraki, M., Basta, M., & Bixler, E. O. (2010). Insomnia with short sleep duration and mortality: The Penn State cohort. Sleep, 33(9), 1159–1164.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Watson, D., Clark, L., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54(6), 1063–1070.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Woods, H., Marchetti, L. M., Biello, S. M., & Espie, C. A. (2009). The clock as a focus of selective attention in those with primary insomnia: An experimental study using a modified Posner paradigm. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 47(3), 231–236. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2008.12.009.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wu, J., & Bunney, W. (1990). The biological basis of an antidepressant response to sleep deprivation and relapse: Review and hypothesis. American Journal of Psychiatry, 147, 14–21.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Yoo, S.-S., Gujar, N., Hu, P., Jolesz, F. A., & Walker, M. P. (2007). The human emotional brain without sleep–a prefrontal amygdala disconnect. Current Biology, 17(20), R877–R878. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2007.08.007.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

The authors would like to thank the Michigan Psychoneuroendocrinology Affective Laboratory staff, particularly Andrew Garton, Rebecca Mulder, Tonia Ballantyne, Lara Fawaz, Allie Hammond, and Rachel Cannon who assisted in data collection. Christopher Drake has received research support from Merck, Jazz, Aladdin Dreamer, Actelion, Pernix, Eisai, and the Robert Wood Johnson Foundation. Christopher Drake has received a speaker honorarium from Merck.

Funding

Funding for this research was provided by the University of Michigan Department of Psychology, University of Michigan Rackham Graduate School, Blue Cross Blue Shield of Michigan Foundation, and the American Psychological Association of Graduate Students. All grants were awarded to IV. The funding source had no role in the study design, data collection, analyses, or manuscript preparation.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Ivan Vargas.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of Interest

Ivan Vargas and Nestor Lopez-Duran declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Ethical Approval

The present study was in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.

Informed Consent

Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.

Animal Rights

No animal studies were carried out by the authors for this article.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Vargas, I., Drake, C.L. & Lopez-Duran, N.L. Insomnia Symptom Severity Modulates The Impact of Sleep Deprivation on Attentional Biases to Emotional Information. Cogn Ther Res 41, 842–852 (2017). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10608-017-9859-4

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10608-017-9859-4

Keywords

Navigation