Cognitive Therapy and Research

, Volume 39, Issue 2, pp 89–99 | Cite as

Implicit Motives, Explicit Motives, and Motive-Related Life Events in Clinical Depression

Original Article

Abstract

Past research suggests that implicit motive dispositions moderate individuals’ affective responses to stimuli and life events and are related to well-being and symptoms of depression. We examined whether this association also extends to clinical depression by comparing patients diagnosed with a depressive disorder (n = 30) with a control group of surgery patients (n = 31) on implicit motives, assessed with a picture-story exercise, explicit motives, assessed via questionnaire, and recall and affect ratings of motive-related positive and negative life events. Depressed patients had lower levels of implicit needs for achievement and power than controls. Differences for implicit affiliation motivation as well as for the corresponding explicit motives were in the same direction, but considerably smaller. Compared to controls, depressed individuals recalled more positive and negative life events, but only rated the latter (particularly in the domains of power and achievement) more negatively. These findings suggest that implicit motive concepts and measures may provide a fruitful approach to understanding depression.

Keywords

Implicit motives Picture-story exercise Need for achievement Need for power Need for affiliation Explicit motives Life events Depression 

Notes

Acknowledgments

The results presented in this paper are based on the first author’s master’s thesis. The data reported in this paper have not previously been presented. We thank Christina Adelhardt for her help in coding PSEs.

Conflict of Interest

Marie-Luise Neumann and Oliver C. Schultheiss declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Informed Consent

All procedures followed were in accordance with the ethical standards of the responsible committee on human experimentation (institutional and national) and with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2000. Informed consent was obtained from all patients for being included in the study.

Animal Rights

No animal studies were carried out by the authors for this article.

References

  1. Abramson, L. Y., Seligman, M. E. P., & Teasdale, J. D. (1978). Learned helplessness in humans: Critique and reformulation. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 87, 49–74.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  2. American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (DSM-IV-TR) (4th ed.). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.Google Scholar
  3. Atkinson, J. W. (1957). Motivational determinants of risk-taking behavior. Psychological Review, 64, 359–372.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  4. Ball, S. G., Otto, M. W., Pollack, M. H., & Rosenbaum, J. F. (1994). Predicting prospective episodes of depression in patients with panic disorder: A longitudinal study. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 62, 359–365.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  5. Bársony, K., Martos, T., Ehmann, B., Balázs, H., & Demetrovics, Z. (2013). Implicit motive profile of treatment-seeking opiate users: High affiliation and low achievement. Journal of Psychoactive Drugs, 45, 374–378.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Bartelstone, J. H., & Trull, T. J. (1995). Personality, life events, and depression. Journal of Personality Assessment, 64, 279–294.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  7. Beck, A. T., Steer, R. A., & Brown, G. K. (1996). Manual for the BDI-II. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
  8. Beck, A. T., Ward, C. H., Mendelson, M., Mock, J., & Erbaugh, J. (1961). An inventory for measuring depression. Archives of General Psychiatry, 4, 561–571.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  9. Bipp, T., & van Dam, K. (2014). Extending hierarchical achievement motivation models: The role of motivational needs for achievement goals and academic performance. Personality and Individual Differences, 64, 157–162.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Blatt, S. J., & Zuroff, D. C. (1992). Interpersonal relatedness and self-definition: Two prototypes for depression. Clinical Psychology Review, 12, 527–562.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Brunstein, J. C. (2008). Implicit and explicit motives. In J. Heckhausen & H. Heckhausen (Eds.), Motivation and action (2nd ed.). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  12. Brunstein, J. C. (2010). Implicit motives and explicit goals: The role of motivational congruence in emotional well-being. In O. C. Schultheiss & J. C. Brunstein (Eds.), Implicit motives (pp. 347–374). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Brunstein, J. C., & Heckhausen, H. (2008). Achievement motivation. In J. Heckhausen & H. Heckhausen (Eds.), Motivation and action (2nd ed., pp. 137–183). New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Brunstein, J. C., Lautenschlager, U., Nawroth, B., Pöhlmann, K., & Schultheiss, O. (1995). Persönliche Anliegen, soziale Motive und emotionales Wohlbefinden [Personal goals, social motives, and emotional well-being]. Zeitschrift für Differentielle und Diagnostische Psychologie, 16, 1–10.Google Scholar
  15. Brunstein, J. C., & Maier, G. W. (2005). Implicit and self-attributed motives to achieve: two separate but interacting needs. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89, 205–222.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  16. Brunstein, J. C., Schultheiss, O. C., & Grässmann, R. (1998). Personal goals and emotional well-being: The moderating role of motive dispositions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 494–508.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  17. Clark, D. A., Steer, R. A., Beck, A. T., & Ross, L. (1995). Psychometric characteristics of revised sociotropy and autonomy scales in college students. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 33, 325–334.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  18. Cohen, J. (1992). A power primer. Psychological Bulletin, 112, 155–159.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  19. Dohrenwend, B. P. (2006). Inventorying stressful life events as risk factors for psychopathology: Toward resolution of the problem of intracategory variability. Psychological Bulletin, 132, 477–495.CrossRefPubMedCentralPubMedGoogle Scholar
  20. Ehlert, U., Gaab, J., & Heinrichs, M. (2001). Psychoneuroendocrinological contributions to the etiology of depression, posttraumatic stress disorder, and stress-related bodily disorders: The role of the hypothalamus-pituitary-adrenal axis. Biological Psychology, 57, 141–152.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  21. Eshel, N., & Roiser, J. P. (2010). Reward and punishment processing in depression. Biological Psychiatry, 68, 118–124.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  22. Fodor, E. M., & Wick, D. P. (2009). Need for power and affective response to negative audience reaction to an extemporaneous speech. Journal of Research in Personality, 43, 721–726.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Fodor, E. M., Wick, D. P., & Hartsen, K. M. (2006). The power motive and affective response to assertiveness. Journal of Research in Personality, 40, 598–610.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Fuhr, K., Hautzinger, M., & Meyer, T. D. (2014). Implicit motives and cognitive variables: Specific links to vulnerability for unipolar or bipolar disorder. Psychiatry Research, 215, 61–68.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  25. Gilbert, P. (2006). Evolution and depression: Issues and implications. Psychological Medicine, 36, 287–297.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  26. Holmes, T. H., & Rahe, R. H. (1967). The social radjustment rating scale. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 11, 213–218.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  27. Joiner, T. E. (2002). Depression in its interpersonal context. In I. H. Gotlib & C. H. Hammen (Eds.), Handbook of depression (pp. 295–313). New York: Guilford.Google Scholar
  28. Kehr, H. M. (2004). Implicit/explicit motive discrepancies and volitional depletion among managers. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30, 315–327.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  29. Kessler, R. C. (1997). The effects of stressful life events on depression. Annual Review of Psychology, 48, 191–214.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  30. Köllner, M., & Schultheiss, O. C. (2014). Meta-analytic evidence of low convergence between implicit and explicit measures of the needs for achievement, affiliation, and power. Frontiers in Psychology. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00826.
  31. Kuhl, J. (2001). Motivation und Persönlichkeit: Interaktionen psychischer Systeme [Motivation and personality: Interactions of mental systems]. Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
  32. Langens, T., Schmalt, H.-D., & Sokolowski, K. (2005). Motivmessung: Grundlagen und Anwendungen [Motive assessment: Basic principles and applications]. In R. Vollmeyer & J. Brunstein (Eds.), Motivationspsychologie und ihre Anwendung (3rd ed., pp. 72–91). Stuttgart: Kohlhammer.Google Scholar
  33. McAdams, D. P., & Vaillant, G. E. (1982). Intimacy motivation and psychosocial adjustment: A longitudinal study. Journal of Personality Assessment, 46, 586–593.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  34. McClelland, D. C. (1987). Human motivation. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  35. McClelland, D. C., Koestner, R., & Weinberger, J. (1989). How do self-attributed and implicit motives differ? Psychological Review, 96, 690–702.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Monroe, S. M., & Hadjiyannakis, K. (2002). The social environment and depression: Focusing on severe life stress. In I. H. Gotlib & C. H. Hammen (Eds.), Handbook of depression (pp. 314–340). New York: Guilford.Google Scholar
  37. Murray, H. A. (1938). Explorations in personality. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  38. Musty, R. E., & Kaback, L. (1995). Relationships between motivation and depression in chronic marijuana users. Life Sciences, 56, 2151–2158.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  39. Orlofsky, J. L. (1978). Identity formation, nAchievement, and fear of success in college men and women. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 7, 49–62.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  40. Pang, J. S., & Schultheiss, O. C. (2005). Assessing implicit motives in U.S. College students: Effects of picture type and position, gender and ethnicity, and cross-cultural comparisons. Journal of Personality Assessment, 85, 280–294.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  41. Patalakh, M., & Schultheiss, O.C. (2010). Motivational life events questionnaire (MLEQ). Department of Psychology, Friedrich-Alexander University, Erlangen, Germany: Unpublished manuscript.Google Scholar
  42. Pizzagalli, D. A., Iosifescu, D., Hallett, L. A., Ratner, K. G., & Fava, M. (2008). Reduced hedonic capacity in major depressive disorder: Evidence from a probabilistic reward task. Journal of Psychiatry Research, 43, 76–87.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Pueschel, O., Schulte, D., & Michalak, J. (2011). Be careful what you strive for: the significance of motive–goal congruence for depressivity. Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy, 18, 23–33.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  44. Reeve, J., Olson, B. C., & Cole, S. G. (1987). Intrinsic motivation in competition: The intervening role of four individual differences following objective competence information. Journal of Research in Personality, 21, 148–170.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Schönbrodt, F. D., & Gerstenberg, F. X. R. (2012). An IRT analysis of motive questionnaires: The unified motive scales. Journal of Research in Personality, 46, 725–742.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Schüler, J., Wegner, M., & Brandstätter, V. (2013). A critical view on implicit and explicit motive measures: What do they have in common and what differentiates them? Paper presented at the 6th annual meeting of the society for the study of motivation.Google Scholar
  47. Schultheiss, O. C. (2008). Implicit motives. In O. P. John, R. W. Robins, & L. A. Pervin (Eds.), Handbook of personality: Theory and research (3rd ed., pp. 603–633). New York: Guilford.Google Scholar
  48. Schultheiss, O. C. (2013). Are implicit motives revealed in mere words? Testing the marker-word hypothesis with computer-based text analysis. Frontiers in Psychology. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00748.
  49. Schultheiss, O. C., & Brunstein, J. C. (2001). Assessing implicit motives with a research version of the TAT: Picture profiles, gender differences, and relations to other personality measures. Journal of Personality Assessment, 77, 71–86.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  50. Schultheiss, O. C., & Brunstein, J. C. (2005). An implicit motive perspective on competence. In A. J. Elliot & C. Dweck (Eds.), Handbook of competence and motivation (pp. 31–51). New York: Guilford.Google Scholar
  51. Schultheiss, O. C., Jones, N. M., Davis, A. Q., & Kley, C. (2008). The role of implicit motivation in hot and cold goal pursuit: Effects on goal progress, goal rumination, and depressive symptoms. Journal of Research in Personality, 42, 971–987.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Schultheiss, O. C., & Köllner, M. (2014). Implicit motives and the development of competencies: A virtuous-circle model of motive-driven learning. In R. Pekrun & L. Linnenbrink-Garcia (Eds.), International handbook of emotions in education (pp. 73–95). New York, NY: Taylor & Francis/Routledge.Google Scholar
  53. Schultheiss, O. C., & Pang, J. S. (2007). Measuring implicit motives. In R. W. Robins, R. C. Fraley, & R. Krueger (Eds.), Handbook of research methods in personality psychology (pp. 322–344). New York: Guilford.Google Scholar
  54. Schultheiss, O. C., Patalakh, M., Rawolle, M., Liening, S., & MacInnes, J. J. (2011). Referential competence is associated with motivational congruence. Journal of Research in Personality, 45, 59–70.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Schultheiss, O. C., & Rohde, W. (2002). Implicit power motivation predicts men’s testosterone changes and implicit learning in a contest situation. Hormones and Behavior, 41, 195–202.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  56. Schultheiss, O. C., & Schiepe-Tiska, A. (2013). The role of the dorsoanterior striatum in implicit motivation: The case of the need for power. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00141.
  57. Schultheiss, O. C., Wiemers, U., & Wolf, O. T. (2014). Implicit need for achievement predicts attenuated cortisol responses to difficult tasks. Journal of Research in Personality, 48, 84–92.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  58. Schultheiss, O. C., Wirth, M. M., Torges, C. M., Pang, J. S., Villacorta, M. A., & Welsh, K. M. (2005). Effects of implicit power motivation on men’s and women’s implicit learning and testosterone changes after social victory or defeat. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 88, 174–188.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  59. Schultheiss, O. C., Yankova, D., Dirlikov, B., & Schad, D. J. (2009). Are implicit and explicit motive measures statistically independent? A fair and balanced test using the Picture Story Exercise and a cue- and response-matched questionnaire measure. Journal of Personality Assessment, 91, 72–81.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  60. Spangler, W. D. (1992). Validity of questionnaire and TAT measures of need for achievement: Two meta-analyses. Psychological Bulletin, 112, 140–154.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  61. Sutton, J. M., Mineka, S., Zinbarg, R. E., Craske, M. G., Griffith, J. W., Rose, R. D., et al. (2011). The relationships of personality and cognitive styles with self-reported symptoms of depression and anxiety. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 35, 381–393.CrossRefPubMedCentralPubMedGoogle Scholar
  62. Weinberger, J., & McClelland, D. C. (1990). Cognitive versus traditional motivational models: Irreconcilable or complementary? In E. T. Higgins & R. M. Sorrentino (Eds.), Handbook of motivation and cognition: Foundations of social behavior (Vol. 2, pp. 562–597). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  63. Winter, D. G. (1991). Measuring personality at a distance: Development of an integrated system for scoring motives in running text. In D. J. Ozer, J. M. Healy, & A. J. Stewart (Eds.), Perspectives in personality (Vol. 3, pp. 59–89). London: Jessica Kingsley.Google Scholar
  64. Winter, D.G. (1994). Manual for scoring motive imagery in running text (4 ed.). Department of Psychology, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor: Unpublished manuscript.Google Scholar
  65. Wirth, M. M., & Schultheiss, O. C. (2006). Effects of affiliation arousal (hope of closeness) and affiliation stress (fear of rejection) on progesterone and cortisol. Hormones and Behavior, 50, 786–795.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  66. Wirth, M. M., Welsh, K. M., & Schultheiss, O. C. (2006). Salivary cortisol changes in humans after winning or losing a dominance contest depend on implicit power motivation. Hormones and Behavior, 49, 346–352.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  67. Woike, B. A. (2008). A functional framework for the influence of implicit and explicit motives on autobiographical memory. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 12, 99–117.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media New York 2014

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Klinikum Chemnitz gGmbHChemnitzGermany
  2. 2.Department of PsychologyFriedrich-Alexander UniversityErlangenGermany

Personalised recommendations