Advertisement

Cognitive Therapy and Research

, Volume 37, Issue 5, pp 1048–1057 | Cite as

‘Folk Theories’ About the Causes of Insomnia

  • Allison G. Harvey
  • Adriane Soehner
  • Tania Lombrozo
  • Lynda Bélanger
  • Jamie Rifkin
  • Charles M. Morin
Original Article

Abstract

The present study investigates ‘folk theories’ about the causes of insomnia. Participants with insomnia (n = 69) completed a qualitative and quantitative assessment of their folk theories. The qualitative assessment was to speak aloud for 1 min in response to: ‘What do you think causes your insomnia?’. The quantitative assessment involved completing the ‘Causal Attributions of My Insomnia Questionnaire’ (CAM-I), developed for this study. The three most common folk theories for both the causes of one’s own insomnia as well as insomnia in others were ‘emotions’, ‘thinking patterns’ and ‘sleep-related emotions’. Interventions targeting these factors were also perceived as most likely to be viable treatments. Seventy-five percent of the folk theories of insomnia investigated with the CAM-I were rated as more likely to be alleviated by a psychological versus a biological treatment. The results are consistent with research highlighting that folk theories are generally coherent and inform a range of judgments. Future research should focus on congruence of ‘folk theories’ between treatment providers and patients, and the role of folk theories in treatment choice, engagement, compliance and outcome.

Keywords

Insomnia Folk theories Causal attributions 

Notes

Acknowledgments

This project was supported by National Institute of Mental Health Grant RO1MH079188. We are grateful to Kerrie Hein & Manon Lamy (Project Co-ordinators), Jennifer Kanady, Jason Lee, Kate Kaplan & Eleanor McGlinchey (Assessors), Lisa S. Talbot, Polina Eidelman, Simon Beaulieu-Bonneau & Émilie Fortier-Brochu (Therapists) and Nicole Short (Coder) for their assistance with this study.

Conflict of Interest

Dr. Morin serves on consulting/advisory boards for Merck, Valeant, Purdue, Novartis, Eli Lilly and the speaker’s bureau for Valeant and Merck. The other authors have no conflict of interest disclosures.

References

  1. Ahn, W., Proctor, C. C., & Flanagan, E. H. (2009). Mental health clinicians’ beliefs about the biological, psychological, and environmental bases of mental disorders. Cognitive Science, 33, 147–182.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Baglioni, C., Lombardo, C., Bux, E., Hansen, S., Salveta, C., Biello, S., et al. (2010). Psychophysiological reactivity to sleep-related emotional stimuli in primary insomnia. Behavior Research and Therapy, 48, 467–475.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Bastien, C. H., Vallieres, A., & Morin, C. M. (2001). Validation of the insomnia severity index as an outcome measure for insomnia research. Sleep Medicine, 2, 297–307.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Carey, S. (1985). Conceptual change in childhood. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  5. Daley, M., Morin, C. M., LeBlanc, M., Gregoire, J. P., & Savard, J. (2009). The economic burden of insomnia: direct and indirect costs for individuals with insomnia syndrome, insomnia symptoms, and good sleepers. Sleep, 32(1), 55–64.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  6. Dweck, C. S. (2000). Self-theories: Their role in motivation, personality, and development. Philadelphia, PA: Taylor & Francis/Psychology Press.Google Scholar
  7. Edinger, J. D., Bonnet, M. H., Bootzin, R. R., Doghramji, K., Dorsey, C. M., Espie, C. A., et al. (2004). Derivation of research diagnostic criteria for insomnia: report of an American Academy of Sleep Medicine Work Group. Sleep, 27, 1567–1596.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  8. Epley, N., Keysar, B., Van Boven, L., & Gilovich, T. (2004). Perspective taking as egocentric anchoring and adjustment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 87, 327–339.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Espie, C. A., Barrie, L. M., & Forgan, G. S. (2012). Comparative investigation of the psychophysiologic and idiopathic insomnia disorder phenotypes: Psychologic characteristics, patients’ perspectives, and implications for clinical management. Sleep, 35(3), 385.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  10. First, M. B., Spitzer, M. B., Gibbon, M., & Williams, J. B. W. (1995). Structured clinical interview for DSM-IV Axis I disorderspatient edition (SCID-I/P, Version 2.0). New York: Biomedics Research Department, New York State Psychiatric Institute.Google Scholar
  11. Glass, C. R., Arnkoff, D. B., & Shapiro, S. J. (2001). Expectations and preferences. Psychotherapy, 38, 455–461.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Goethals, G. R., Messick, D. M., & Allison, S. T. (1991). The uniqueness bias: Studies of constructive social comparison. In J. Suls & T. A. Will (Eds.), Social comparison: Contemporary theory and research (pp. 149–176). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  13. Gopnik, A., & Meltzoff, A. (1997). Words, thoughts and theories. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  14. Hajak, G., Petukhova, M., Lakoma, M. D., Coulouvrat, C., Roth, T., Sampson, N. A., et al. (2011). Days-out-of-role associated with insomnia and comorbid conditions in the america insomnia survey. Biological Psychiatry, 70, 1063–1073.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Halford, W. K., Keefer, E., & Osgarby, S. M. (2002). “How has the week been for you two?” Relationship satisfaction and hindsight memory biases in couples reports of relationship events. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 26, 759–773.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Harvey, A. G., & Bryant, R. A. (1999). A qualitative investigation of the organization of traumatic memories. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 38, 401–405.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Harvey, A. G., McGlinchey, E., & Gruber, J. (2009). Toward an affective science of insomnia treatments. In A. Kring & D. Sloan (Eds.), Emotion regulation and psychopathology (pp. 427–446). Guilford.Google Scholar
  18. Harvey, A. G., Stinson, K., Whitaker, K. L., Moskovitz, D., & Virk, H. (2008). The subjective meaning of sleep quality: A comparison of individuals with and without insomnia. Sleep, 31(3), 383–393.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  19. Iacoviello, B. M., McCarthy, K. S., Barrett, M. S., Rynn, M., Gallop, R., & Barber, J. P. (2007). Treatment preferences affect the therapeutic alliance: Implications for randomized controlled trials. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 75, 194–198.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Kempton, W. (1986). Two theories of home heat control. Cognitive Science, 10, 75–90.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Kim, N. S., & Ahn, W. (2002a). Clinical psychologists’ theory-based representations of mental disorders predict their diagnostic reasoning and memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 131, 451–476.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Kim, N. S., & Ahn, W. (2002b). The influence of naive causal theories on lay concepts of mental illness. American Journal of Psychology, 115, 33–65.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. King, M., Nazareth, I., Lampe, F., Bower, P., Chandler, M., Morou, M., et al. (2005). Impact of participant and physician intervention preferences on randomized trials: A systematic review. Journal of the American Medical Association, 293, 1089–1099.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Kocsis, J. H., Leon, A. C., Markowitz, J. C., Manber, R., Arnow, B., Klein, D. N., et al. (2009). Patient preference as a moderator of outcome for chronic forms of major de- pressive disorder treated with nefazodone, cognitive behavioral analysis system of psychotherapy, or their combination. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 70, 354–361.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Kring, A. M. (2009). Emotion regulation and psychopathology: A transdiagnostic approach to etiology and treatment. Guilford: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  26. Kring, A. M., & Werner, K. H. (2004). Emotion regulation and psychopathology. In P. Philippot & R. S. Feldman (Eds.), The regulation of emotion (pp. 359–385). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Publishers.Google Scholar
  27. Kruger, J. (1999). Lake Wobegon be gone! The “below-average effect” and the egocentric nature of comparative ability judgments. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77, 221–222.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Kwan, B. M., Dimidjian, S., & Rizvi, S. L. (2010). Treatment preference, engagement, and clinical improvement in pharmacotherapy versus psychotherapy for depression. Behavior Research and Therapy, 48, 799–804.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Lichstein, K. L., & Rosenthal, T. L. (1980). Insomniacs’ perceptions of cognitive versus somatic determinants of sleep disturbance. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 88, 105–107.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Lombrozo, T. (2012). Explanation and abductive inference. In K. J. Holyoak & R. G. Morrison (Eds.), Oxford handbook of thinking and reasoning (pp. 260–276). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  31. Morin, C. M., Belleville, G., Bélanger, L., & Ivers, H. (2011). The insomnia severity index: Psychometric indicators to detect insomnia cases and evaluate treatment response. Sleep, 34, 601–608.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  32. Morin, C. M., Gaulier, B., Barry, T., & Kowatch, R. A. (1992). Patients’ acceptance of psychological and pharmacological therapies for insomnia. Sleep, 15, 302–305.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  33. Murphy, G. L., & Medin, D. L. (1985). The role of theories in conceptual coherence. Psychological Review, 92, 289–316.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Nickerson, R. S. (1999). How we know—and sometimes misjudge—what others know: Imputing one’s own knowledge to others. Psychological Bulletin, 125, 737–759.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Seligman, M. E. (1991). Learned optimism. New York: Knopf.Google Scholar
  36. Seligman, M. E. (1995). The effectiveness of psychotherapy. The consumer reports study. American Psychologist, 50, 965–974.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Skre, I., Onstad, S., Torgersen, S., & Kringlen, E. (1991). High interrater reliability for the structured clinical interview for DSM-III–R axis I (SCID-I). Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 84, 167–173.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Swift, J. K., & Callahan, J. L. (2009). The impact of client treatment preferences on outcome: A meta-analysis. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 65, 368–381.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Vincent, N., & Lionberg, C. (2001). Treatment preference and patient satisfaction in chronic insomnia. Sleep, 24, 411–417.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  40. Wellman, H., & Gelman, S. A. (1988). Children’s understanding of the nonobvious. In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.), Advances in the psychology of human intelligence (Vol. 4). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  41. Williams, J. B., Gibbon, M., First, M. B., Spitzer, R. L., Davis, M., Borus, J., et al. (1992). The structured clinical interview for DSM-III-R (SCID): Multisite test-retest reliability. Archives of General Psychiatry, 49, 630–636.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media New York 2013

Authors and Affiliations

  • Allison G. Harvey
    • 1
  • Adriane Soehner
    • 1
  • Tania Lombrozo
    • 1
  • Lynda Bélanger
    • 2
  • Jamie Rifkin
    • 3
  • Charles M. Morin
    • 2
  1. 1.Department of PsychologyUniversity of CaliforniaBerkeleyUSA
  2. 2.École de PsychologieUniversité LavalQuebecCanada
  3. 3.Los Angeles County Department of Mental HealthLos AngelesUSA

Personalised recommendations